Introduction
The issue of specificity impairment in depressed individuals remains fiercely debated. Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a common psychiatric disorder with uncertain etiology, which is accompanied by several symptoms, such as depressed mood and loss of interest. Frequent reports exist regarding cognitive impairments in people with MDD, including executive impairments as well as attention and memory deficits [
1, 2]. These cognitive impairments prevent the improvement of depression as they increase relapse episodes [
3]. From an etiological perspective, shrinking hippocampus and prefrontal cortical grey matter volume result in some of these cognitive impairments [
4, 5]. In depressed populations, memory and attention impairments are more prominent [
6]. Mood-congruent recall, impaired memory specificity (over general autobiographical memory), and intrusive memories are three alterations in the memory functioning of depressed people, which have been frequently reported [
7]. In this paper, we investigated the underlying mechanisms for memory specificity impairment. Autobiographical memory is defined as a memory system that stores our life experiences and maintains information in three levels: Lifetime periods, general memories, and specific memories. Specificity impairment occurs when individuals fail to complete the hierarchical retrieval process from lifetime periods to specific memories and retrieval is subsequently truncated prior to the activation of specific memories [
8].
Specificity impairment leads to problem-solving disabilities in people with MDD [
9], which in turn indicates the importance of examining underlying mechanisms for memory specificity impairment.
Executive resource framework
One proposed explanation for individual differences in autobiographical memory recall lies in the executive resource hypothesis. Conway and Pleydell-Pearce’s model (2000) assumes that the limitation of working memory capacity results in reduced specificity so that executive capacity is required for model definition retrieval and maintenance, as well as to avoid the interference of autobiographical information. This assumption relies on some arguments proposed in the cognitive resource models of depression. A proposed explanation for the resource allocation model states that depressed individuals should be offered an optimal task completion strategy because they lack initiative during task execution [
10]. In Conway and Pleydell-Pearce’s model (2000), an efficient search description is required to map the memory space to be searched. The lack of initiative is, therefore, likely to hinder this process. Another supporting theory states that depressed individuals have inhibition problems during cognitive task performance [
11]. In the capture and rumination theory, which will be discussed in the following section, it is easier to conceptualize how inhibition deficits contribute to specificity impairment.
Capture and rumination framework
In this framework, it is believed that many intermediate descriptions, which are applied to assist autobiographical memory recall are based on self-representations [
12]. When people experience rumination, like people with MDD, the dominance of self-representation results in specificity impairment and truncation of retrieval. Failure in completing the retrieval process also occurs once individuals have negative self-schemas [
13]. As cue words presented in the autobiographical memory test are emotional words, they activate negative self-beliefs and deter depressed people from completing the hierarchical retrieval process. The search process is compromised when people lack decent inhibition ability. In this case, emotional cue words lead to the activation and accumulation of irrelevant information raised by negative self-beliefs and consequently, result in the excessive occupation of working memory [
13].
Current study
In this study, we administered the ruminative response scale (RRS) as well as the forward and backward digit span test to examine whether subtypes of rumination and working memory capacity predict specificity impairment. There is contradictory evidence for the supporting role of working memory in memory specificity [
14 ,15], which encouraged us to conduct the current study.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Our sample size consisted of 105 adults with MDD, 53(50.5%) males and 52(49.5%) females, referred to a psychiatric clinic in Sari City between January and February 2021 using the convenience sampling method. Each participant was informed about the study aims and procedures before the study and consent forms were signed by participants. Patients aged 20 to 45 years who met the inclusion criteria of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition, were included. The inclusion criterion was MDD adults aged between 20 and 45. We also established exclusion criteria, including people younger than 20 and older than 45 years old, people with comorbidity and substance use disorder, amnestic patients, and people who did not sign the consent form. For diagnosis purposes, two clinicians examined the patients and used structured clinical interviews as well as the second edition of the Beck depression inventory (BDI) to make diagnoses.
Materials and procedure
The current study is correlational research, in which we administered the following questionnaires as well as autobiographical memory interviews to collect the data. We distributed the ruminative response scale, BDI, and autobiographical memory test (AMT). Participants also performed the digit span working memory test and autobiographical memory interviews.
Beck depression inventory (BDI-II)
The second version of the self-report BDI consists of 21 items and allows one to recognize the severity of depression in adults and its items are scored on four various degrees–from low degree to intensive.
Autobiographical memory test (AMT)
The interview was used to measure the capability of retrieving specific memories in response to cue words. To clarify the feature concept, we explained to the participants that the recalled memories should be memories that happened in one day and they should try to remember the people, objects, environmental features, etc. involved in the memory.
Ruminative response scale (RRS)
RRS is a self-report questionnaire that consists of 22 items and measures the depressive mood and rumination in individuals. Each item is evaluated on a 4-point Likert scale, which allows for measuring the severity of rumination more precisely. The total score ranges from 22 to 88, with higher scores indicating higher degrees of ruminative symptoms.
Forward and backward digit span test
Short-term memory capacity was measured by the digit span forward and backward tasks from the Wechsler memory scale-revised. The two tasks were operated separately. In the digit span forward task, number sequences were presented to the patients and they were asked to repeat the numbers immediately. After every successful attempt, numbers were increased until individuals failed to repeat numbers in the same order. After two consecutive unsuccessful attempts, the test was stopped and the last sequence of numbers was considered the score for this part of the experiment. The backward section of the test required participants to recall the number sequence inversely. Measuring the backward digit span was the same as the forward digit span.
Data analysis
Initially, we recorded the demographic information and used descriptive statistics, including Mean±SD, and minimum and maximum scores to describe them. Then, to test our hypotheses, we used the Spearman correlation coefficient and multiple regression analysis.
Results
Our samples consisted of 105 adults with MDD (53(50.5%) males and 52(49.5%) females). It is also worth mentioning that our data lacked any missing values to exclude from the sample. This enabled us to enter the entire sample for analysis. To have a comprehensive demographic profile of the participants, we also took the marital status and age of the participants into account: 31 cases (29.5%) were single, 64 cases (61.0%) were married and, ten cases (9.5%) were divorced. Also, 30 cases (28.6%) belonged to the 20-29 age group, 61 cases (58.1%) to 30-39, and 14 cases (13.3%) to the 40-49 age group. The participants’ mean age was 33.38 years with a range of 22-43 years.
The Mann-Whitney U test showed no significant difference between males and females based on memory specificity (P=0.438). The Kruskal-Wallis test indicated no significant difference between single, married, and divorced participants (P=0.860) as well as between various age groups in terms of memory specificity (P=0.235). The demographic characteristics of participants as well as the results of the Mann-Whitney U and Kruskal-Wallis tests are demonstrated in
Table 1.
The descriptive statistics, including the Mean±SD and minimum and maximum values for rumination (including brooding and reflection), working memory capacity (both forward and backward digit span), and memory specificity were calculated. The results provided in
Table 2 show a difference in the mean values of the subscales of rumination.
The difference in the mean values also existed in the forward and backward digit span tasks. Descriptive statistics of research variables indicated that the means working memory capacity, brooding, reflection, and memory specificity scores were 7.38±1.38, 15.31±2.56, 12.51±2.13, and 2.17±0.48, respectively. Descriptive statistics of the research variables are illustrated in
Table 2.
After data analyses, we tested our research hypotheses. After checking the assumptions for parametric and nonparametric statistics, including normality of distribution, the existence of outliers, linearity of the relationship, etc. We used the Spearman correlation coefficient to examine the correlation between research variables. The non-parametric statistic was performed given that the normality assumption was not met for all the research variables. This means that not all research variables follow a normal distribution and the skewness is statistically significant. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to check the normality of the data.
Our data analysis determined a significant direct relationship between working memory capacity and memory specificity (r=0.216, P<0.05). However, our analysis did not show a significant correlation between memory specificity and rumination (r=-0.157, P>0.05). To determine the correlation between subtypes of rumination and memory specificity, we also examined the correlation coefficient between brooding, reflection, and memory specificity. As shown in
Table 3, no meaningful relationship was found between neither brooding and memory specificity (r=0.086, P<0.05) nor reflection and memory specificity (r=-0.074, P<0.05).
Multiple regression was conducted to examine how working memory capacity and rumination can predict memory specificity levels. By examining the scatter plot and linearity of the relationship, we ensured that our data met the assumptions of the model. We also checked the outliers and made sure that there were no outliers in the data. Examining the power of working memory capacity and rumination in the prediction of autobiographical memory specificity determined that only working memory capacity could predict memory specificity and rumination could not predict our dependent variable. A detail of the regression model is demonstrated in
Table 4.
Discussion
The results of our study showed that working memory capacity could predict specificity impairment in people with MDD. This is consistent with studies that demonstrated the relationship between executive deficits and specificity problems [
15-
18]. All these findings support the executive resource hypothesis. In line with this hypothesis, it should be stated that depression is accompanied by executive deficits, which result in memory impairments. These memory impairments are due to the structural and functional alterations of the brain in the depressed group. Widespread alterations occur in several brain regions, including the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex as regions associated with executive, cognitive, and emotional processing [
19-
21]. In addition, decreasing grey matter volume in the prefrontal cortex is another factor involved in executive deficits. As the Carfax model emphasized, working memory capacity is not only required to maintain, define, and hold the optimal retrieval model but it is also crucial for inhibiting interfering autobiographical information.
The credibility of the executive resource framework is pronounced by taking other executive processes into account. In other words, the cumulative effects of these executive impairments can lead to specific retrieval deficits. For instance, inhibition disability has been determined in people with MDD [
22]. Any deficits in inhibiting irrelevant information during retrieval lead to the occupation of memory capacity. The lack of capacity caused by a cascade of irrelevant thoughts leads to an inefficient process of mnemonic items associated with episodes [
13], which in turn leads to specificity impairment. This attentional deficit can occur at any point during the retrieval process. To have a better understanding of this process, two types of autobiographical memory retrieval should be differentiated. The retrieval of autobiographical events occurs in two ways: Direct and generative retrieval [
23]. Direct retrieval happens as a result of the immediate activation of specific memories. This retrieval strategy is an effortless process that only occurs when mnemonic items that are related to the memory are accessible and when memories are not emotionally demanding. However, when one of these two conditions is not met, episodes are retrieved as a result of generative retrieval, which is a top-down process, in which memories are activated through a hierarchical retrieval pattern from the lifetime events to specific memories [
14]. Given the emotional nature of the words, which are presented during the autobiographical memory test, memories are most likely recalled through generative retrieval. Therefore, inhibition problems at any stage of the generative retrieval can occur and result in a limited capacity. In our previous study, we demonstrated the relationship between inhibitory control and specificity impairment [
24].
In our current investigation, we also intended to determine if subtypes of rumination can be regarded as irrelevant information, which is activated during generative retrieval. There was no relationship between either subtype of rumination and specificity impairments. Inconsistent results exist regarding the relationship between brooding and reflection rumination with specificity impairment in depressed groups. While the Carfax model did not differentiate between subtypes of rumination as a predictor of specificity impairment, several investigations have demonstrated a correlation between brooding and specificity problems [
25-
28]. However, our results are consistent with some studies indicating no meaningful relationship between subtypes of rumination and memory specificity [
29, 30]. Concerning these contradictory results, it can be concluded that the capture and rumination framework is not as credible as the executive resource hypothesis to predict specificity impairments. To conceptualize how memory capacity is occupied, we can return to the executive resource framework instead of relying on the capture and rumination theory. Working memory capacity can be occupied as a result of ineffective search strategies instead of ruminative thoughts. As described by affect-as-information theory, information is processed by either interpretive or detailed processing depending on the mood. Once a low mood is experienced, detailed processing is adopted, in which attention is divided into details instead of concepts. Conversely, when individuals experience high mood, they apply interpretive processing, in which attentional resources are devoted more efficiently and memory capacity is less likely to be occupied by irrelevant or peripheral details [
31].
These results convinced us to observe reduced specificity as a result of either avoidance strategies or executive deficits. Along with the executive resource hypothesis, active avoidance can also play a part as an underlying mechanism for reduced specificity. Although depressed people are always susceptible to intrusive memories, they are typically inclined to avoid thinking about past events because they have negative interpretations of these events, which in turn enhances negative emotions (functional avoidance theory) [
13]. This avoidance can result in the overgenerality of memories.
Future research can investigate the interaction of other executive functions and specificity impairment to support our results. As there is a strong interplay between attention and working memory, we encourage future researchers to examine the interplay of various types of attentional control, working memory and reduced specificity to conceptualize the underlying mechanisms for this retrieval type more efficiently and methodically.
Considering the importance of specific memories for problem-solving and treatment purposes, we highly encourage using neuropsychological treatments to improve executive functions for specificity purposes in MDD people. Our results determined the crucial role of executive capacity for memory specificity and convinced us to observe proper executive functions as a prerequisite for recalling specific memories. It is worth mentioning that we never overlooked the importance of other therapy approaches, such as cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) or mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) to prevent avoidance coping strategies. Our recommendation is to use the combination of neuropsychological treatments with other types of treatments, especially cognitive treatments to address specificity impairment in this population. Psychological treatments have been developed and used in Iran for patients with mental health issues [
32-
34]. Such treatments need to be evaluated and used for depressed patients with memory problems in the Persian context.
Conclusion
Considering our results, limited executive capacity deters depressed people from retrieving specific memories, Although rumination is a characteristic of depression, neither brooding nor reflection rumination can result in reduced specificity.
Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Mazandaran University of Medical Science (Code: IR.MAZUMS.REC.1401.9319).
Funding
This research did not receive any grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or non-profit sectors.
Authors' contributions
All authors equally contributed in preparation of the paper.
Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank all participants in the study. In addition, the authors are grateful to Seyed Hamzeh Hosseini for providing us with all participants and a suitable environment for conducting research.
References
- McIntyre RS, Xiao HX, Syeda K, Vinberg M, Carvalho AF, Mansur RB, et al. The prevalence, measurement, and treatment of the cognitive dimension/domain in major depressive disorder. CNS Drugs. 2015; 29(7):577-89. [DOI:10.1007/s40263-015-0263-x] [PMID]
- Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brüne M, Bullmore ET, Carter CS, Clayton NS, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews. Drug Discovery. 2012; 11(2):141-68. [DOI:10.1038/nrd3628] [PMID]
- Lee RS, Hermens DF, Porter MA, Redoblado-Hodge MA. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode Major Depressive Disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012; 140(2):113-24. [DOI:10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.023] [PMID]
- Malykhin NV, Carter R, Seres P, Coupland NJ. Structural changes in the hippocampus in major depressive disorder: Contributions of disease and treatment. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience. 2010; 35(5):337-43. [DOI:10.1503/jpn.100002] [PMID]
- Salvadore G, Nugent AC, Lemaitre H, Luckenbaugh DA, Tinsley R, Cannon DM, et al. Prefrontal cortical abnormalities in currently depressed versus currently remitted patients with major depressive disorder. Neuroimage. 2011; 54(4):2643-51. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.011] [PMID]
- Chamberlain SR, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychology of mood disorders. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2006; 8(6):458-63. [DOI:10.1007/s11920-006-0051-x] [PMID]
- Whalley MG, Rugg MD, Brewin CR. Autobiographical memory in depression: An fMRI study. Psychiatry Research. 2012; 201(2):98-106. [DOI:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.08.008] [PMID]
- Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review. 2000; 107(2):261-88. [DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.261] [PMID]
- Cuijpers P, de Wit L, Kleiboer A, Karyotaki E, Ebert DD. Problem-solving therapy for adult depression: An updated meta-analysis. European Psychiatry. 2018; 48:27-37. [DOI:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.11.006] [PMID]
- Hertel PT, Hardin TS. Remembering with and without awareness in a depressed mood: Evidence of deficits in initiative. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1990; 119(1):45-59. [DOI:10.1037/0096-3445.119.1.45] [PMID]
- Zacks, RT, Lynn H. Directed ignoring: Inhibitory regulation of working memory. In: Dagenbach D, Carr TH, editors. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. Cambridge: Academic Press; 1994. [Link]
- Conway MA. Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language. 2005; 53(4):594-628. [DOI:10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005]
- Williams JM, Barnhofer T, Crane C, Herman D, Raes F, Watkins E, et al. Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin. 2007; 133(1):122-4. [DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122] [PMID]
- Ros L, Latorre JM, Serrano JP. Working memory capacity and overgeneral autobiographical memory in young and older adults. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. 2010; 17(1):89-107. [DOI:10.1080/13825580903042650] [PMID]
- Stewart TM, Hunter SC, Rhodes SM. A prospective investigation of rumination and executive control in predicting overgeneral autobiographical memory in adolescence. Memory & Cognition. 2018; 46(3):482-96. [DOI:10.3758/s13421-017-0779-z] [PMID]
- Dalgleish T, Williams JM, Golden AM, Perkins N, Barrett LF, Barnard PJ, et al. Reduced specificity of autobiographical memory and depression: The role of executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2007; 136(1):23-42. [DOI:10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.23] [PMID]
- Guler OE, Mackovichova S. The role of executive function in autobiographical memory retrieval: Does the type of cue word matter? Memory. 2019; 27(4):423-30. [DOI:10.1080/09658211.2018.1513040] [PMID]
- Piolino P, Coste C, Martinelli P, Macé AL, Quinette P, Guillery-Girard B, et al. Reduced specificity of autobiographical memory and aging: do the executive and feature binding functions of working memory have a role? Neuropsychologia. 2010; 48(2):429-40. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.035] [PMID]
- Hare BD, Duman RS. Prefrontal cortex circuits in depression and anxiety: Contribution of discrete neuronal populations and target regions. Molecular Psychiatry. 2020; 25(11):2742-58. [DOI:10.1038/s41380-020-0685-9] [PMID]
- Sheline YI, Liston C, McEwen BS. Parsing the hippocampus in depression: Chronic stress, hippocampal volume, and major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2019; 85(6):436-8. [DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.01.011] [PMID]
- Yang TT, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Tapert SF, Frank GK, Max JE, et al. Adolescents with major depression demonstrate increased amygdala activation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010; 49(1):42-51. [DOI:10.1016/j.jaac.2009.09.004] [PMID]
- Shimony O, Einav N, Bonne O, Jordan JT, Van Vleet TM, Nahum M. The association between implicit and explicit affective inhibitory control, rumination and depressive symptoms. Scientific Reports. 2021; 11(1):11490. [DOI:10.1038/s41598-021-90875-3] [PMID]
- Meléndez JC, Redondo R, Escudero J, Satorres E, Pitarque A. Executive functions, episodic autobiographical memory, problem-solving capacity, and depression proposal for a structural equations model. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology. 2019; 32(2):81-9. [DOI:10.1177/0891988718824037] [PMID]
- Rahgosha P, Abdollahzadeh H, Hosseini SH. [Study of the relationship between working memory capacity and inhibitory control with overgeneral autobiographical memory in adults with major depressive disorder (Persian)]. Paper presented at: 10th International Conference on Psychology, Counseling and Educational Sciences. 26 November 2021; Tehran, Iran. [Link]
- Liu Y, Yu X, Yang B, Zhang F, Zou W, Na A, et al. Rumination mediates the relationship between overgeneral autobiographical memory and depression in patients with major depressive disorder. BMC Psychiatry. 2017; 17(1):103. [DOI:10.1186/s12888-017-1264-8] [PMID]
- Ricarte J, Ros L, Serrano JP, Martínez-Lorca M, Latorre JM. Age differences in rumination and autobiographical retrieval. Aging & Mental Health. 2016; 20(10):1063-9. [DOI:10.1080/13607863.2015.1060944] [PMID]
- Ricarte JJ, Ros L, Latorre JM, Muñoz MD, Aguilar MJ, Hernandez JV. Role of anxiety and brooding in specificity of autobiographical recall. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2016; 57(6):495-500. [DOI:10.1111/sjop.12323] [PMID]
- Romero N, Vazquez C, Sanchez A. Rumination and specificity of autobiographical memory in dysphoria. Memory. 2014; 22(6):646-54. [DOI:10.1080/09658211.2013.811254] [PMID]
- Chiu CPY, Griffith JW, Lenaert B, Raes F, Hermans D, Barry TJ. Meta-analysis of the association between rumination and reduced autobiographical memory specificity. Memory. 2018; 26(10):1323-34. [DOI:10.1080/09658211.2018.1474928] [PMID]
- Smets J, Griffith JW, Wessel I, Walschaerts D, Raes F. Depressive symptoms moderate the effects of a self-discrepancy induction on overgeneral autobiographical memory. Memory. 2013; 21(6):751-61. [DOI:10.1080/09658211.2012.756039] [PMID]
- Clore G, Wyer RS, Dienes B, Gasper K. Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In: Clore GL, Martin LL, editors. Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2001. [Link]
- Dana S, Effatpanah M, Mahjoub A. The new epidemic problem of psychoactive drugs at drug treatment centers of Iran: Implications for education, prevention and treatment. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry Behavioral Sciences. 2018; 12(2):e63555. [DOI:10.5812/ijpbs.63555]
- Effatpanah M, Moradi A. Methamphetamine dependence and technology-based interventions in Iran. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry Behavioral Sciences. 2018; 12(2):e62935. [DOI:10.5812/ijpbs.62935]
- Sami S, Effatpanah M, Moradi A, Massah O. Matrix model as an intensive rehabilitation in three methadone services in Iran. Iranian Rehabilitation Journal. 2017; 15(3):293-8. [DOI:10.29252/nrip.irj.15.3.293]