Volume 18, Issue 3 (September 2020)                   Iranian Rehabilitation Journal 2020, 18(3): 257-262 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Azad O. The Analysis of Semantic Field in Persian-Speaking Patients With Wernicke’s Aphasia. Iranian Rehabilitation Journal 2020; 18 (3) :257-262
URL: http://irj.uswr.ac.ir/article-1-1102-en.html
Department of English, Faculty of Linguistics, University of Gonabad, Gonabad, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 545 kb]   (855 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (1702 Views)
Full-Text:   (664 Views)
1. Introduction
The semantic field or semantic domain describes words based on their similar semantic features [1]. The words in a semantic field encompass a general phenomenon. Being a general layer of meaning, the semantic field coordinates words based on their common conceptual framework [2, 3]. Because of its significant importance in the linguistic processing of healthy individuals, some studies have delved into its internal structure. Among these research studies, some are outstanding [4,5,6,7].
It is noteworthy to mention that the studies of semantic processing are not confined to healthy individuals. That is, the analysis of semantic processing in neuropsychological diseases like aphasia has always been taken seriously by researchers. Aphasia is a neuropsychological and neuropathological disease that is the result of focal damage to the temporal or frontal lobes of the brain. In this disease, people’s comprehensive and productive abilities are damaged affecting their linguistic capabilities more particularly. In this way, While Broca’s aphasia affects the productive abilities of people, Wernicke’s aphasia negatively affects the comprehensive abilities of people. The different areas of language which are negatively affected in aphasia include semantic processing, advanced syntactic structure, pragmatic and phonetic abilities, semantic processing, and in particular semantic field analysis. They have been considered in previous literature both in patients with Boca’s [8,9,10,11,12] and Wernicke’s aphasia [13,14,15,16].
Although some papers investigated the internal structure of the semantic field of patients with Wernicke in different languages, few studies have concentrated on this issue in the Persian context. For instance, Mastura et al. have analyzed semantic processing in Persian participants [17]. Although in these studies, semantic abilities in neuropsychological and neurodegenerative diseases have been investigated, the analysis of semantic field structure and more importantly its internal structure has not been taken into account seriously.
As a dynamic semantic phenomenon and also due to its intercultural differences in different languages and its relationship to belief systems [3], the semantic field should be investigated in different languages like Persian and in doing so, a more vivid picture of the nature of its probable deficit could be drawn. In Iran, no specific research has yet studied the structure of the semantic field in aphasia. Hence, because of the lack of enough research on analyzing the nature of semantic processing in pathological diseases in general and Persian-speaking patients with Wernicke’s aphasia in particular, this study tried to fill in the aforementioned gap in the Persian setting via scrutinizing an important area of language namely semantics. In doing so, we suggest some possible linguistic strategies which could be used to enhance patients’ communicative competence. So, the rehabilitative outcome of the research could be robust.

2. Methods
Initially, 15 monolingual Persian-speaking male patients with Wernicke’s aphasia were chosen from Mehr Hospital, of whom 12 were chosen following the order of their admission to the hospital. The specific tool to select these patients was Boston Aphasia Battery Subtests of Auditory Comprehension. In the next stage, 2 patients were eliminated from the test because they were too impaired to understand and perform the test accurately. 
Ultimately,10 patients with severe Wernicke’s aphasia and 20 age- and education-matched healthy controls were chosen after consulting with their doctors and reviewed their neuropsychological profile. In the case of both groups, written consent was taken based on which all subjects unanimously accepted to take part in the research. The inclusion criteria for the Wernicke’s group included: 1. Lack of addiction to alcohol or other drugs; 2. Lack of any other focal damage or any kind of neuropsychological diseases like severe depression or anxiety; 3. Intact visual or comprehension ability in such a way that the participants could perform satisfactorily on the experiment. 
The primary test used to assess subjects’ semantic network status was the semantic priming test. The rationale for choosing this test was its viability, easy performance, and manipulation. To choose our test’s tokens, first of all, 40 flashcards denoting some frequently used nouns and adjectives were chosen. In the next stage, to select appropriate tokens, the reliability and validity of the words were assessed via asking 50 age- and education-matched individuals to select as many semantic associative types as possible based on the pictures drawn on different flashcards. Having participated in the pilot study, all healthy individuals could choose all semantic associative pictures with high reliability (95%). Hence, all the stimuli were chosen for our main test. Before initiating the test, all subjects were instructed on how to conduct the test. That is, all subjects were explained that they were to judge which picture stimuli corresponds to which target word(s). The semantic associative sub-types to be chosen for analysis and their definitions were shown in Table 1.
Having enumerated the number of correct associative priming that the participants could count and also their performance pattern, the final data were obtained and subsequently analyzed using inferential statistics, including the independent t-test, Duncan and 2-way ANOVA. 

3. Results
In Table 2, the percentage of correct associative pictures of each semantic associative subtype has been shown.

As the Table 2 shows, there were significant differences between patients with Wernicke’s aphasia and healthy controls both in the number of correct responses they could produce and also in the optimal performance in each semantic subtype. 
In Table 3, the subjects’ performance in each associative semantic subtype was compared using the independent t-test, Duncan and 2-way ANOVA. Moreover, to compare healthy controls and healthy patients with Wernicke’s aphasia in each subcategory, the Pearson correlation coefficient was used. 
 
As the above Table shows, a systematic difference in the structure of semantic associations in either group could easily be detected. That is, while the first semantic associative subtypes chosen by patients with Wernicke’s aphasia were identity and contrast coordinates, the healthy controls’ priorities in semantic subtype recognition were function and attribute subtypes. 

4. Discussion
The purpose of our paper was twofold. First, we were going to see if the structure of the semantic field is damaged in severe Wernicke’s aphasia and if so, more specifically, and as our second objective, which component(s) of the semantic field was or were probably more severely damaged. 
Our results showed that patients with Wernicke’s aphasia compared to their healthy counterparts had problems in enumerating sufficient associative semantic categories. That is, much smaller associative lists they could count compared to those of healthy controls on the one hand and also their tendency to choose some semantically unrelated pictures for the target words, on the other hand, corroborates our hypothesis asserting Wernicke’s semantic field deficit. This result, consistent with the related literature, demonstrates Wernicke’s patients’ deficits in the semantic field [13,14,15,16, 18, 19]. Moreover, our research corroborates with Rose and Douglas’s study suggesting that knowledge about object attribute and function are among the most determining parameters in Wernicke’s patients’ semantic field internal structure [20]. Their results showed that semantic field analysis could be an important tool to recognize lexical-semantic impairment in aphasic patients.
Moreover, patients with Wernicke’s aphasia tend to choose an identity as their foremost semantic associative subtype on the one hand, and also their preference to opt functional context semantic associative subtype as their ultimate category on the other hand. All provide reliable evidence justifying the deficit not only in the quantity of semantic network but also in its quality proving more fundamental problems in these patients. As Thompson and his colleagues assert, when functional context subtypes of the semantic field are more severely damaged, it will be a defining factor demonstrating semantic field breakdown [16]. In this circumstance, their major semantic deficit could be attributed to storage rather than retrieval representing a more critical semantic processing deficit in these patients. 

5. Conclusion
This research as the first in its type tried to shed some light on the nature of the semantic field in two groups of patients and healthy control. As the findings showed, because of damage to the temporal lobe and prefrontal areas of the brain, individuals’ semantic fields were damaged interfering with their linguistic performance. A lot of research studies highlight the importance of semantic network and intact semantic ability in both comprehending and producing linguistic stimuli that have so far been analyzed [4-7]. Because of the critical role of the semantic field, speech therapists and neuropsychologists, being aware of this issue, could analyze the structure of the semantic field in Wernicke’s aphasia and in doing so, find the results of such studies useful in employing suitable rehabilitative strategies. The outcome of these measures would be Wernicke’s patients’ better performance in different linguistic contexts. However, as our ultimate comment, it should also be emphasized that our research was conducted on a highly restricted group of patients with Wernicke’s aphasia whose number was also limited. Thus, to achieve more comprehensive and conclusive results, other similar complementary studies with more participants, and also more demographic variables like social class or gender could also be taken into consideration. 
Last but not least, it is through all these considerations that a more realistic picture of Wernicke’s aphasia could be depicted.

Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines
All ethical principles are considered in this article. The participants were informed about the purpose of the research and its implementation stages.

Funding
This research did not receive any grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or non-profit sectors. 

Conflict of interest
The author declared no conflict of interest.

References
  1. Brinton LJ, Brinton D. The linguistic structure of modern English. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing; 2010. [DOI:10.1075/z.156]
  2. Akmajian A, Demers RA, Harnish RM. Linguistics: An introduction to language and communication. 2th ed. London: MIT Press; 1984.
  3. Hintikka J. Aspects of metaphor. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media; 2013. https://books.google.com/books/about/Aspects_of_Metaphor.html?id=gx1e2gJFWPgC
  4. Benítez PF, Usón RM. The paradigmatic and syntagmatic structure of the lexical field of feeling. Cuadernos de Investigación Filológica. 2013; 23:35-60. [DOI:10.18172/cif.2407]
  5. Johnson-Laird PN, Oatley K. The language of emotions: An analysis of a semantic field. Cognition and Emotion. 1989; 3(2):81-123. [DOI:10.1080/02699938908408075]
  6. Nerlich B, Clarke DD. Semantic fields and frames: Historical explorations of the interface between language, action, and cognition. Journal of Pragmatics. 2000; 32(2):125-50. [DOI:10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00042-9]
  7. Zhouravlev A, Dobrova V, Nurtdinova L. On some basic methods of analysis of content and structure of lexical-semantic fields. International Conference” Topical Problems of Philology and Didactics: Interdisciplinary Approach in Humanities and Social Sciences”(TPHD 2018). Paris: Atlantis Press; 2019. [DOI:10.2991/tphd-18.2019.89]
  8. Butterworth B, Howard D, McLoughlin P. The semantic deficit in aphasia: The relationship between semantic errors in auditory comprehension and picture naming. Neuropsychologia. 1984; 22(4):409-26. [DOI:10.1016/0028-3932(84)90036-8] [PMID]
  9. Grober E. The breakdown of word meanings in aphasia. Cortex. 1984; 20(4):557-66. [DOI:10.1016/S0010-9452(84)80058-1] [PMID]
  10. Grodzinsky Y, Amunts K. Broca’s region. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006. [DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195177640.001.0001]
  11. Semenza C, Bisiacchi PS, Romani L. Naming disorders and semantic representations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 1992; 21(5):349-64. [DOI:10.1007/BF01067920] [PMID]
  12. Yee E, Blumstein SE, Sedivy JC. Lexical-semantic activation in Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008; 20(4):592-612. [DOI:10.1162/jocn.2008.20056] [PMID] [PMCID]
  13. Balliet R, Levy B, Blood KM. Upper extremity sensory feedback therapy in chronic cerebrovascular accident patients with impaired expressive aphasia and auditory comprehension. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1986; 67(5):304-10. [PMID]
  14. Piñango MM, Zurif EB. Semantic Operations in Aphasic Comprehension: Implications for the Cortical Organization of Language. Brain and Language. 2001; 79(2):297-308. [DOI:10.1006/brln.2001.2492] [PMID]
  15. Pulvermuller F, Mohr B, Sedat N, Hadler B, Rayman J. Word class-specific deficits in Wernicke’s aphasia. Neurocase. 1996; 2(3):203-12. [DOI:10.1093/neucas/2.3.203-a]
  16. Thompson HE, Robson H, Lambon Ralph MA, Jefferies E. Varieties of semantic ‘access’ deficit in Wernicke’s aphasia and semantic aphasia. Brain. 2015; 138(12):3776-92. [DOI:10.1093/brain/awv281] [PMID] [PMCID]
  17. Akhmedova MN, Garayeva AK. General issues of homonymy in the Persian language. Journal of Sustainable Development. 2015; 8(4):126-31. [DOI:10.5539/jsd.v8n4p126]
  18. Bisiacchi P, Denes G, Semenza C. Semantic field in aphasia: An experimental investigation on comprehension of the relations of class and property. Schweizer Archiv Für Neurologie, Neurochirurgie and Psychiatrie. 1976; 118(2):207-13. [PMID]
  19. Melice-Ledent S, Gainotti G, Messerli P, Tissot R. Elementary logic and semantic fields in aphasia. Revue Neurologique. 1976; 132(5):343-59. [PMID]
  20. Rose M, Douglas J. Treating a semantic word production deficit in aphasia with verbal and gesture methods. Aphasiology. 2008; 22(1):20-41. [DOI:10.1080/02687030600742020]
Article type: Original Research Articles | Subject: Speech therapy
Received: 2019/11/27 | Accepted: 2020/01/20 | Published: 2020/09/1

Send email to the article author


Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb