Volume 22, Issue 4 (December 2024)                   Iranian Rehabilitation Journal 2024, 22(4): 573-582 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Sheikh M, Hosseini H A. The Influence of Fear of Falling on Cognitive-Motor Interference in Walking in Older Adults. Iranian Rehabilitation Journal 2024; 22 (4) :573-582
URL: http://irj.uswr.ac.ir/article-1-2049-en.html
1- Dynamic Movement Physical Therapy Center, Mashhad, Iran.
2- Department of Physical Therapy, School of Paramedical and Rehabilitation Sciences, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 569 kb]   (65 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (1099 Views)
Full-Text:   (23 Views)
Introduction
The fear of falling (FOF) is a prevalent condition among older people, characterized by low perceived self-assurance or self-efficacy [1]. The frequency of FOF varies from 21% to 85% among the elderly population with past falls and from 35% to 46% among those without prior falls [2]. Due to activity restriction or avoidance, FOF can lead to physical and psychological impairments [3]. Limiting activities may result in muscular weakness, postural impairments, deconditioning, and potential future falls [3]. In addition, FOF has been related to decreased ability to perform activities of daily living (ADL), reduced quality of life (QoL), and increased mortality [3, 4].
Earlier studies have demonstrated a complex interplay between FOF, falls, and walking impairments in older adults [5, 6, 7]. FOF is related to increased gait variability, possibly increasing instability and fall risk [7]. Previous studies have shown that FOF results in an inability to allocate attention effectively to walking control while dual-task conditions in aged society [8, 9]. However, the impact of FOF on motor or cognitive function during dual-tasking is unclear in this population. 
In real daily life, walking is not performed as an isolated activity. In most conditions, gait is combined with additional motor or cognitive tasks (dual-task conditions) [10]. Several studies revealed increased gait variability and reduced step length during dual-tasking in older people [11, 12, 13]. All these impairments have been considered indicators of increased fall risk [10]. In dual-task conditions, older adults may decrease performance in mobility, cognitive, or both tasks due to increased cognitive demands [14]. Schaefer and Schumacher reported that during perturbed standing and walking, older people prioritize mobility tasks over cognitive tasks [15]. Fallers also prioritize walking during dual-task conditions compared to non-fallers [15]. 
Notably, optimal execution of mobility and cognitive tasks is imperative for a safe and functional gait [14]. Since FOF interferes with attention to maintain balance control [8], people with FOF may have greater cognitive-motor interference than those without FOF. Hence, the objective of the present investigation was to ascertain the impact of FOF on the interference of dual tasks in walking with the aged community. The current results may help plan interventions for older people to walk more safely. It was hypothesized that older adults with functional impairment would exhibit greater cognitive-motor interference than those without it. Furthermore, it was anticipated that an increase in FOF levels would correlate with increased cognitive-motor interference. 

Materials and Methods
Study participants 

The present study was cross-sectional. Ninety community-dwelling older adults from Mashhad City, Iran, were recruited using a convenience sampling. Based on an initial power analysis (α error probability=0.05, power=0.8, and effect size=0.07), 86 participants were necessary for adequate statistical power. A researcher in the field screened all participants based on the eligibility criteria. The inclusion criteria included age 65-80, the ability to walk 20 m without walking aids, and mini-mental state examination scores >24 to reject any cognitive impairment [16]. The exclusion criteria included severe health problems (uncontrolled hypertension or uncontrolled diabetes), vision and hearing problems, neurologic or orthopedic diseases with documented influence on gait control, and an inability to give informed consent. Mashhad University of Medical Sciences approved the study protocol, and informed consent was provided from all participants.
The falls efficacy scale international (FES-I) was utilized to evaluate FOF. The FES-I instrument measures the level of FOF throughout 16 physical and social activities [17, 18]. The determination of the level of FOF for each activity is based on a 4-point scale (1: No FOF to 4: High FOF). The higher score demonstrates more FOF. The score range is between 16 and 64. This questionnaire is a valid and reliable instrument for evaluating the degree of FOF in older adults [17]. According to the level of concern about falls, all participants were divided into three groups: High FOF, low FOF, and no FOF (30 participants in each group). A score of 16 demonstrates no FOF [18]. A score of 23 was considered the threshold for allocating subjects into high and low FOF groups [18]. Subjects with scores below 23 were considered low FOF groups, and subjects above 23 were considered high FOF groups [18]. The number of falls during the last year was also determined for each participant in the present study. For this purpose, a fall was defined as falling unintentionally on the floor or ground, and falls due to a medical condition or an external force were excluded from the analysis [19]. 

Cognitive-motor interference in walking
In the dual-task testing, three distinct flexibility tasks with different degrees of hardship (elementary, moderate, complex) were executed, especially forward walking (FW), timed up and go (TUG), and obstacle crossing (OC) [20]. During the FW assessment, the subjects were instructed to walk on a 15-m walkway. In the TUG test, the participants were required to rise from a chair, walk for a distance of 3 m, and then return to their initial position. A stopwatch determined the completion time. In the OC test, 7 obstacles (length 70 cm, height 4 cm, width 6 cm) were embedded in the 15-m walkway. Obstacles were located 1.5 m apart from each other. Only the walking time in the middle 10 m of the 15-m walkway was determined for FW and OC. A shorter duration of the test exhibited superior functional performance.
Two cognitive tasks were assessed: Verbal fluency (VF) and mental tracking (MT). During the VF test, the subjects were instructed to mention specific words about a distinct category, such as vegetables. In the MT test, subjects count 4-additions consecutively from a number between 50 and 60. The valid response numbers were determined. During single-tasking, the subjects were instructed to begin with one of the motor tasks and then perform the same motor task plus one of the randomized cognitive tasks in a dual-task situation. Finally, subjects executed cognitive tasks independently. Dual-task evaluations demonstrated good reliability [20]. Each participant performed one practice trial before recording the data. 
The rate of the correct answering to cognitive tasks was determined as described below (Equation 1):

1. Correct answer rate (CAR)=(Number of correct answers/time)×100
The percentage of dual-task interference (DTI%) was calculated as described below (Equations 2 and 3):

2. DTI% in walking time: (Walking duration during dual tasking–walking duration during single-tasking)÷ (walking duration during single-tasking)×100
3. DTI% in CAR: (CAR during single tasking–CAR during dual tasking) ÷ (CAR during single-tasking) ×100

A higher value of these variables demonstrated higher dual-task interference (higher damage to the performance during dual-tasking rather than single-tasking) [21]. 

Statistical analysis
Participant characteristics were described using descriptive statistics. Data normality was determined based on the Shapiro-Wilk test. The distinction in baseline measurements between the groups was ascertained using either the Kruskal-Wallis test or a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). The analysis of variance with Bonferroni correction was employed to determine the disparities in dual-task interference measures among the groups. The homogeneity and normality of variance were also checked. Effect sizes were shown by ηp2 values. A 95% confidence interval (CI) was considered the disparity in any parameter between the groups. The relationships between the dual-task interference measures and FES-I scores in participants with FOF were determined using the Pearson correlation coefficients. The statistical test was executed employing SPSS software, version 19 (available in Chicago, IL) for Windows platforms. The α level was set at 0.05.

Results
Table 1 presents the general characteristics of all subjects.


The differences in demographic characteristics between the three groups were not significant. The distribution of all measures was normal according to the Shapiro-Wilk test. 
The Mean±SD outcome measures in the 3 groups are shown in Table 2.


The assumptions, including the homogeneity and normality of variance, were not violated. 
The results of multivariate tests revealed no significant distinctions between the three groups regarding the composed variables (P=0.001; F=11.8; ηp2=0.724). The results of between-subject effects that consider the dependent variables separately identified statistically significant difference in DTI% in CAR (MT+TUG) (P=0.001; F=11.504; ηp2=0.209), DTI% in CAR (VF+OC) (P=0.001; F=12.1; ηp2=0.218), DTI% in CAR (MT+OC) (P=0.001; F=7.729; ηp2=0.151), and fall numbers (P=0.001; F=36.674; ηp2=0.457) between the three groups. The difference in other dependent variables was not statistically significant between the three groups (Table 2).
The results of pair group comparisons identified the statistically significant difference in DTI% in CAR (MT+TUG) between high FOF and no FOF groups (95% CI, -19.432%, -12.249%, P=0.01), and high FOF and low FOF groups (95% CI, 9.226%, 20.364%, P=0.02). The statistically significant difference was also identified in DTI% in CAR (VF+OC) between high FOF and no FOF groups (95% CI, -18.436%, -0.624%; P=0.03), and high FOF and low FOF groups (95% CI, 9.03%, 26.843%; P=0.041). The difference in DTI% in CAR (MT+OC) was statistically different between high FOF and no FOF groups (95% CI, 6.951%, 13.871%; P=0.001), and high FOF and no FOF groups (95% CI, 3.069%, 19.988%; P=0.01). There was also a statistically significant difference in fall numbers between no FOF and low FOF groups (95% CI, -6.033%, -0.634%; P=0.01), high FOF and no FOF groups (95% CI, -9.833 %, -5.434%; P=0.001), and high FOF and low FOF groups (95% CI, -6%, -2.6%; P=0.001).
In participants with FOF, a significant positive relationship was seen between FES-I grades and DTI% in CAR (MT+TUG) measures (r=0.76, P<0.001) (Figure 1), FES-I scores and DTI% in CAR (VF+OC) measures (r=0.72, P<0.001) (Figure 2), and also between FES-I scores and DTI% in CAR (MT+OC) measures (r=0.65, P<0.001) (Figure 3).


The correlation among FES-I scores and other dual-task interference measures was not significant.

Discussion
The principal goal of the current investigation was to figure out the cognitive-motor interference experienced by older people with different grades of FOF. This study also investigated the associations between cognitive-motor interference measures and FES-I in older persons with FOF. The current results demonstrated a significant difference in cognitive performance between the three groups during dual-task conditions (DTI% in CAR), including MT+TUG, VF+OC, and MT+OC. FES-I score was also positively related to DTI% in CAR (MT+TUG), DTI% in CAR (VF+OC), and DTI% in CAR (MT+OC) measures in participants with FOF.
 According to current results, all mobility performances during dual-task conditions (DTI% in walking) were not different between the three groups. Walking is not an isolated daily activity; it is mostly part of a dual-task or multi-task performance [22]. The satisfactory attention to each simultaneous performance allows subjects to walk safely in various physical environments [23]. Based on a previous study, older people prioritize mobility performance over cognitive tasks during balance disturbances [13]. This finding has mainly been reported in walking and standing tasks [13]. Older people may adopt mobility task prioritization to improve their ability to balance maintenance and diminish the possibility of falling during dual-task execution [13]. Based on the current results, older people with FOF may still prioritize walking to maintain their physical performance.
Current results identified differences in some cognitive performances during dual-task conditions (DTI% in CAR) between the three groups. Cognitive performance during simple mobility tasks such as FW did not change; however, during more complicated mobility tasks, including TUG and OC, a significant difference in cognitive performance was identified between no FOF and high FOF groups and between low FOF and high FOF groups. Walking is not just an automated and cyclic process; it needs attention, which increases with age and task complexity [23]. Moreover, ignoring inappropriate information about the environment is also crucial for functional walking [9]. It has been proposed that concern about falling in older people may interfere with attention resources and reduce the ability to inhibit inappropriate inputs from surroundings [14]. During dual-task conditions, the amount of attention needs to be higher for mobility and cognitive tasks to manage both performances similarly [8]. Based on the current results, older people with high FOF and limited attentional resources may decrease their cognitive performance to maintain their capability to perform more challenging mobility tasks like TUG and OC. In the present study, VF performance during TUG was not different between the three groups; however, MT performance during TUG was different between no FOF and high FOF groups and between low FOF and high FOF groups. More challenging cognitive tasks, such as MT, are more likely to be impaired during dual-task conditions in the high FOF group.
Fall numbers during the last year were different between the three groups. Previous studies revealed that FOF may reduce the ability to adapt to the surrounding environment during walking, resulting in heightened instability and the risk of falling [6, 7]. Based on the current results, broadening the amount of FOF may increase the fall total in older people. 
The correlational analysis demonstrated three significant relationships between cognitive-motor interference measures and FES-I scores in older adults with FOF. Although a causal relationship between these variables cannot be defined from the present study, these results may indicate the influence of FOF on cognitive-motor performance in the aged population. Therefore, it may be necessary to explore the consequence of a decreased FOF on dual-task performance in the elderly community. While this study indicates that there may be a relationship between FOF and cognitive-motor performance, the underlying mechanisms of this association are unclear. Neuroanatomical evidence indicated several pathways between the frontal cortex and subcortical areas, which may explain the influence of emotions on walking performance [24]. A previous study demonstrated more FOF and dysfunction of the frontal cortex and subcortical area in older people with increased gait variability compared to a control group of similar age and sex [24]. According to these results, cortical dysfunction due to FOF may influence walking performance.
There were limitations regarding the current study. First, all participants in this study had no cognitive disorder. The high prevalence of cognitive disorders in older adults might limit the generalization of the current results. Second, the information about falls was retrospectively collected in this study. Many people may not remember the exact number of falls during the last year, so a recall bias may affect fall assessment in the current study. Third, group assignment in this study was based on the FES-I score.
For this reason, the assessor could not be blinded to group assignments, which may increase the risk of bias. Fourth, several potential physical impairments due to aging, including loss of endurance, flexibility, weakness, balance, and neuromuscular impairments, may influence the ability of individuals to perform the tests in this study. Unfortunately, these potential confounders were not measured in the current study. Further investigations should explore the impact of these confounders on the association between FOF and cognitive-motor performance. 
Despite these limitations, the FOF was evaluated using FES-I, a comprehensive questionnaire that provides information regarding the FOF level compared to a dichotomous FOF statement (yes or no) [1718]. The mini-mental state examination was a reliable and valid tool for evaluating cognitive impairment. It was also used for screening cognitive impairment [16]. Cognitive-motor tasks were also performed with different difficulty levels to supply robust comparisons. 
According to the prevailing outcomes, it is beneficial for therapists to consider FOF as a tool for assessing walking performance in older adults. Cognitive behavioral therapy was distinguished as a practical approach to diminishing FOF in the older population [25]. As walking is frequently a component of a dual-task or multi-task activity, it is recommended that more studies invesate the impact of cognitive behavioral therapy on dual-task performance in elderly individuals with FOF.

Conclusion
The current results indicate that cognitive performance during dual tasks in walking may be impaired in older people with high FOF levels. Future clinical trials may be needed to investigate whether reduced FOF has the advantage of dual-task improvement in the elderly population. 

Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines

This study was approved by Mashhad University of Medical Sciences (Code: IR.MUMS.REC.1398.165).

Funding
Mashhad University of Medical Sciences supported the current study.

Authors' contributions
Conceptualization, investigation, writing, review, and editing: Mania Sheikh; Methodology, and writing the original draft: All authors; Funding acquisition, resources, and supervision: Hossein Asghar Hosseini.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank all the subjects who participated the study.

References
  1. Gambaro E, Gramaglia C, Azzolina D, Campani D, Molin AD, Zeppegno P. The complex associations between late life depression, fear of falling and risk of falls. A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Research Reviews. 2022; 73:101532. [DOI:10.1016/j.arr.2021.101532] [PMID]
  2. de Souza LF, Canever JB, Moreira BS, Danielewicz AL, de Avelar NCP. Association between fear of falling and frailty in community-dwelling older adults: A systematic review. Clinical Interventions in Aging. 2022; 17:129-40. [DOI:10.2147/CIA.S328423] [PMID]
  3. Lee SP, Hsu YW, Andrew L, Davis T, Johnson C. Fear of falling avoidance behavior affects the inter-relationship between vision impairment and diminished mobility in community-dwelling older adults. Physiotherapy Theory and Practice. 2022; 38(5):686-94. [DOI:10.1080/09593985.2020.1780656] [PMID]
  4. Qin Y, Li J, McPhillips M, Lukkahatai N, Yu F, Li K. Association of fear of falling with frailty in community-dwelling older adults: A cross-sectional study. Nursing & Health Sciences. 2021; 23(2):516-24. [DOI:10.1111/nhs.12840] [PMID]
  5. Chen WC, Li YT, Tung TH, Chen C, Tsai CY. The relationship between falling and fear of falling among community-dwelling elderly. Medicine (Baltimore). 2021; 100(26):e26492.[DOI:10.1097/MD.0000000000026492] [PMID]
  6. Ayoubi F, Launay CP, Annweiler C, Beauchet O. Fear of falling and gait variability in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association. 2015; 16:14-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.jamda.2014.06.020] [PMID]
  7. Ayoubi F, Launay CP, Kabeshova A, Fantino B, Annweiler C, Beauchet O. The influence of fear of falling on gait variability: Results from a large elderly population-based cross-sectional study. Journal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation. 2014; 11:128. [DOI:10.1186/1743-0003-11-128] [PMID]
  8. Young WR, Mark Williams A. How fear of falling can increase fall-risk in older adults: Applying psychological theory to practical observations. Gait & Posture. 2015; 41(1):7-12.[DOI:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.09.006] [PMID]
  9. Lacour M, Bernard-Demanze L, Dumitrescu M. Posture control, aging, and attention resources: Models and posture-analysis methods. Neurophysiologie Clinique. 2008; 38(6):411-21. [DOI:10.1016/j.neucli.2008.09.005] [PMID]
  10. Wollesen B, Mattes K, Schulz S, Bischoff LL, Seydell L, Bell JW, et al. Effects of Dual-Task management and resistance training on gait performance in older individuals: A randomized controlled trial. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2017; 9:415. [DOI:10.3389/fnagi.2017.00415] [PMID]
  11. Wollesen B, Voelcker-Rehage C, Willer J, Zech A, Mattes K. Feasibility study of dual-task-managing training to improve gait performance of older adults. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research. 2015; 27(4):447-55. [DOI:10.1007/s40520-014-0301-4] [PMID]
  12. Springer S, Giladi N, Peretz C, Yogev G, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual-tasking effects on gait variability: The role of aging, falls, and executive function. Movement Disorders: Official Journal of The Movement Disorder Society. 2006; 21(7):950-7. [DOI:10.1002/mds.20848] [PMID]
  13. Montero-Odasso M, Muir SW, Speechley M. Dual-task complexity affects gait in people with mild cognitive impairment: the interplay between gait variability, dual tasking, and risk of falls. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2012; 93(2):293-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.apmr.2011.08.026] [PMID]
  14. Beurskens R, Bock O. Age-related deficits of dual-task walking: A review. Neural Plasticity. 2012; 2012:131608. [DOI:10.1155/2012/131608] [PMID]
  15. Schaefer S, Schumacher V. The interplay between cognitive and motor functioning in healthy older adults: Findings from dual-task studies and suggestions for intervention. Gerontology. 2011; 57(3):239-46. [DOI:10.1159/000322197] [PMID]
  16. Tuijl JP, Scholte EM, de Craen AJ, van der Mast RC. Screening for cognitive impairment in older general hospital patients: Comparison of the Six-Item Cognitive Impairment Test with the Mini-Mental State Examination. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2012; 27(7):755-62. [DOI:10.1002/gps.2776] [PMID]
  17. Yardley L, Beyer N, Hauer K, Kempen G, Piot-Ziegler C, Todd C. Development and initial validation of the Falls Efficacy Scale-International (FES-I). Age and Ageing. 2005; 34(6):614-9. [DOI:10.1093/ageing/afi196] [PMID]
  18. Delbaere K, Close JC, Mikolaizak AS, Sachdev PS, Brodaty H, Lord SR. The Falls Efficacy Scale International (FES-I). A comprehensive longitudinal validation study. Age and Ageing. 2010; 39(2):210-6. [DOI:10.1093/ageing/afp225] [PMID]
  19. Rossat A, Fantino B, Nitenberg C, Annweiler C, Poujol L, Herrmann FR, et al. Risk factors for falling in community-dwelling older adults: which of them are associated with the recurrence of falls? The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging. 2010; 14(9):787-91. [DOI:10.1007/s12603-010-0089-7] [PMID]
  20. Yang L, He C, Pang MY. Reliability and validity of dual-task mobility assessments in people with chronic stroke. Plos One. 2016; 11(1):e0147833. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0147833] [PMID]
  21. Plummer-D'Amato P, Cohen Z, Daee NA, Lawson SE, Lizotte MR, Padilla A. Effects of once weekly dual-task training in older adults: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Geriatrics & Gerontology International. 2012; 12(4):622-9. [DOI:10.1111/j.1447-0594.2011.00825.x] [PMID]
  22. Faulkner KA, Redfern MS, Cauley JA, Landsittel DP, Studenski SA, Rosano C, et al. Multitasking: Association between poorer performance and a history of recurrent falls. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2007; 55(4):570-6. [DOI:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01147.x] [PMID]
  23. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Movement Disorders. 2008; 23(3):329-42; quiz 472. [DOI:10.1002/mds.21720] [PMID]
  24. Herman T, Giladi N, Gurevich T, Hausdorff JM. Gait instability and fractal dynamics of older adults with a “cautious” gait: Why do certain older adults walk fearfully? Gait and Posture. 2005; 21(2):178-85. [DOI:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2004.01.014] [PMID]
  25. Parry SW, Bamford C, Deary V, Finch TL, Gray J, MacDonald C, et al. Cognitive-behavioural therapy-based intervention to reduce fear of falling in older people: therapy development and randomised controlled trial - the Strategies for Increasing Independence, Confidence and Energy (STRIDE) study. Health Technology Assessment. 2016; 20(56):1-206.[DOI:10.3310/hta20560] [PMID]

 
Article type: Original Research Articles | Subject: Physiotherapy
Received: 2023/08/15 | Accepted: 2024/08/17 | Published: 2024/12/20

Send email to the article author


Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb