Volume 23, Issue 2 (June 2025)                   Iranian Rehabilitation Journal 2025, 23(2): 153-164 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Jafarzadeh H, Haresabadi F, Ghayoumi-Anaraki Z, Abbasi Shaye Z, Maleki Shahmahmood T, Hadadi Aval M. Phonological Measurement Indices in Hearing Impaired Children: Cochlear Implantation and Hearing Aids. Iranian Rehabilitation Journal 2025; 23 (2) :153-164
URL: http://irj.uswr.ac.ir/article-1-2098-en.html
1- Department of Speech Therapy, School of Paramedical and Rehabilitation Sciences, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran.
2- Clinical Research and Development Unit, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran.
3- Department of Audiology, School of Paramedical and Rehabilitation Sciences, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 544 kb]   (59 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (2151 Views)
Full-Text:   (16 Views)
Introduction
Early listening experiences play a crucial role in the acquisition and correct production of the sounds of the native language [1]; therefore, even slight deficits in the sense of hearing can result in problems in the acquisition of various areas of language particularly, phonological skills [2]. Given the prevalence of hearing loss, estimated at 1 to 9 per thousand worldwide, and its negative impact on the development of speech and language skills, it is valuable to conduct studies focusing on the phonological development of affected children [3]. Moeller et al. reported that hearing-impaired children have poor consonant and syllabic development, which can affect the impact of a child’s first words [4]. Children with hearing impairment produce fewer multi-syllable consonant-vowel strings [5].
Many methods analyze children’s phonological development. These include the order of acquisition of speech sounds [6], the number of phonemes in a child’s phonetic repertoire [7], and suppression of phonological processes [8]. Most experimental research focuses on the process of development and acquisition of speech sounds or different segments of the word [9-11], including the percentage of consonants correct (PCC) index, which deals with the correctness of consonants [12], and the percentage of vowel correct (PVC), which refers to the estimation of the percentage of correctly pronounced vowels in speech. However, from a theoretical perspective, children’s ability to correct word production has been identified as a crucial aspect of phonological development. Ingram proposed the phonological mean length of utterance (PMLU) index as a phonological measurement method to assess the phonological complexity of words in speech [13]. This index is calculated by considering word length and PCC [14]. The critical difference between this index and other indices is that it focuses more on consonants to show that children’s errors are more likely to occur with these [15]. 
Children have different types of phonological errors, each affecting speech intelligibility differently. The PMLU index is more sensitive to omission errors than the PCC index. With PCC, no difference is observed between omission and substitution errors, whereas with PMLU, these two error types are calculated differently [14]. Ingram explains the phonological index of whole-word proximity (PWP), an indirect index of word intelligibility that demonstrates the relationship between a child’s correct production and standard production. The PWP index has maximum values when the target word and the word produced by the child match [16].
The proportion of whole-word correctness (PWC) is another index of phonological development at the word level [17]. This is a simple measure to evaluate the overall accuracy of children’s productions compared to adults’ productions [16]. Despite PVC and PCC, whole-word phonological measures, such as PMLU, PWP, and PWC, consider the production accuracy of sounds at the word level and do not consider speech sounds individually. Therefore, these measures can determine a child’s phonological abilities at different stages of growth and can be used to assess progress toward treatment goals. The whole word indices presented by Ingram have been applied to normal children [18], bilingual children [19, 20], longitudinal growth development [9], premature children with production problems [21], and hearing-impaired children [14, 22, 23].
Consistent with the study of children’s phonological development based on whole-word phonological indices, Taelman et al. found that Dutch children’s PMLU scores increased with age, which was predictable because children use more complex words. In addition, the authors argued that the PMLU value reflects phonological-morphological development [24]. Consistent with Ingram, PMLU reference values vary across languages due to the cross-linguistic diversity of phonological characteristics [16]. Maggie et al. concluded that the PWP and PMLU indices can be used to assess and appropriately select words for intervention in children younger than three years old [9].
Some studies have investigated phonological development in hearing-impaired children. Zanichelli found that the PCC index was higher in children with normal hearing than in children with hearing loss in all three tasks: Naming pictures, imitation, and spontaneous speech [25]. Schauwers et al. concluded that the performance of Dutch children with cochlear implants (CI) was weaker on whole-word phonological indices than that of their normal-hearing peers. He also found that earlier implantation in the first year of life led to higher PMLU scores [26]. These results confirm the findings of Moeller et al. who showed that children with hearing loss experience a delay in the development of consonants and syllables, which can hinder the learning of first words [4].
Since the correct production of consonants and vowels can affect speech intelligibility, Yi conducted a study in which the speech intelligibility of the spontaneous speech of 21 children with mild to profound hearing loss was examined and assessed by 46 adults. The results showed that hearing-impaired children’s PVC and PWP indices increased with age. Finally, data analysis showed a significant correlation between whole-word phonological measures and speech intelligibility, and the PCC, PVC, PMLU, and PWP indices were strong predictors of speech intelligibility [23].
Despite the crucial role that whole word-level indices play in determining the development of phonological skills, the differentiation of various phonological disorders [27, 28], and the development of intervention plans for hearing-impaired children and other children with special needs, studies on phonological skills have mainly focused on phonological development at the consonant and vowel levels. Therefore, this study compared the scores of 3-6-year-old Persian-speaking children with hearing loss with their typically developing peers, including PMLU, PWP, PWC, PCC, and PVC.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This cross-sectional study included 100 Persian-speaking children aged 3-6 years, including 25 children with severe hearing loss and hearing aids, 25 with CI, and 50 with normal hearing. Children with hearing aids and CI were selected by convenience sampling from the Naghma and Parvaneha Rehabilitation Center for hearing-impaired children in Mashhad City, Iran. The participants were matched based on age and sex. Children with normal hearing were also selected from kindergartens in Mashhad City in such a way that they were matched with each of the children in the disorder groups in terms of age and sex, and they also met the inclusion criteria.
The inclusion criteria for children with normal hearing included Persian-speaking and monolingual children aged 3-6 years, with normal hearing, no history of otitis media, no history of drug consumption, no co-morbidities, normal development of speech and language skills based on the reports of parents and teachers, the health records available in kindergarten, and the evaluation of the researcher during communication with the child. The inclusion criteria for hearing impaired children included the presence of bilateral sensorineural hearing loss, having bilateral hearing aids for children with hearing aids, unilateral CI for children with CI, at least 1 year has passed since the use of CI and hearing aids, children with hearing aids with severe hearing loss, the occurrence of hearing loss before the age of language learning (before the age of 3), using a hearing aid before the age of language learning (before the age of 3), age range of 3 to 6 years, the history of rehabilitation in the field of hearing training and speech therapy immediately after receiving the hearing aid, not suffering from comorbidities, normal intelligence, being Persian-speaking and monolingual according to the information available in the hearing loss center. The exclusion criteria included the child’s non-cooperation, damage to the hearing aid, concurrent rehabilitation, and the occurrence of any symptoms indicating that the child did not use the hearing aid. 

Procedures
After selecting each subject and obtaining informed consent from their parents, the phonological subtest of the diagnostic evaluation of articulation and phonology-Persian version (DEAP), called the Persian phonological test, was administered to each participant [29]. The validity and reliability of this test have been determined by Zarifian et al. and the test was administered to each participant [29]. The phonological subtest of the DEAP version test includes two parts: Picture naming and description. The picture-naming task included 54 pictures, all with the target consonant in two initial and final positions and all vowels in their syllabic construction. Among the words assigned to this part were 31 monosyllabic items, 18 two-syllable items, four three-syllable items, and one four-syllable item, and 29 words had final consonant clusters. The picture description task included three animated pictures with 14 target words from the picture naming task.
The test was performed after preparing and establishing proper communication with participants in a proper room regarding noise, light, temperature, and ventilation. During the test, the examiner sat in front of the child, and pictures were placed on the table in front of the child. Following implementing the first task (picture naming), the child was provided appropriate behavioral and verbal feedback. After a short rest, the second task (picture description) was completed. Executing the tasks took about 60 minutes, depending on the cooperation and tolerance of the children, and 15 minutes of rest time was between the two tasks. If signs of fatigue were observed on the child’s face, the test was stopped and performed in a different session on the same day.
The participants’ voices were recorded while performing the tasks of picture naming and picture describing. Then the broad phonetic transcription and five phonological indices of PMLU, PWP, PWC, PCC, and PVC were calculated. A single trained speech and language pathologist (SLP) conducted the tests and transcribed and analyzed the recorded samples. Table 1 presents the methods of calculating the indices. To ensure the accuracy of transcriptions and analysis, 10% of the recorded samples, including 10 samples, were randomly selected, separately transcribed, and re-scored by the examiner and a second SLP, and the phonological indices were calculated. 



The intraclass correlation coefficient was used to calculate the intra-rater and inter-rater reliability. A coefficient >70% was considered acceptable and >90% was considered very acceptable [30]. In this study, the intra-rater and inter-rater reliability were >90%. Therefore, the reliability coefficients were acceptable.

Data analysis
SPSS software, version 22 was used for data analysis. Descriptive statistics were used to obtain the Mean±SD median, minimum, and maximum values. Regarding the non-normal distribution of the data specified by the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, the Kruskal-Wallis test was used to compare the investigated phonological indices among the studied groups. The Wilcoxon test was used to compare these indices between the two tasks, including picture naming and description. Since the difference between the groups was significant, linear regression was used to control for confounding variables, such as age and sex.

Results 
This study included 100 children aged 3-6 with normal or impaired hearing. Table 2 presents the participants’ demographic characteristics.



Figure 1 shows the descriptive data related to the phonological indices, including PMLU, PWP, PWC, PCC, and PVC, in three groups and for both picture naming and picture description tasks.



According to Table 3, a comparison of the five phonological indices in the three studied groups using the Kruskal-Wallis test showed significant differences in the mean scores among the three groups of children with hearing aids, CI, and normal hearing (P<0.05). The group with normal hearing had better performance than children with hearing aids and CI, and children with CI had better performance than those with hearing aids.



Table 4 compares phonological indices between the two task of picture naming and description in each group. In the group of children who used hearing aids, a significant difference was observed between the phonological indices extracted from the two different tasks (P<0.05). These children scored better on the PWP and PWC indices in the picture-naming task and the PMLU, PCC, and PVC indices in the picture-description task. In the group of cochlear-implanted children, only the mean scores related to PMLU and PVC indices in the two tasks were significantly different (P<0.05), and they had better performance in both indices in the picture description task. In the group of children with normal hearing, the mean PMLU and PWC scores significantly differed between the two tasks (P<0.05). PMLU was higher in the picture description task, and the PWC index had higher scores in the picture-naming task. 



A linear regression test was used to eliminate the effects of confounding variables, including sex and age (Table 5). The results showed that, considering the confounding variables, this model could predict more than 68% of all variables, except for the PVC index, which was approximately 20%.



Discussion
This study compared the PMLU, PWP, PWC, PCC, and PVC indices among three groups of children: Those with CI, those with hearing aids, and those with normal hearing. The comparison was conducted across two tasks: Picture naming and description. The results showed that children with normal hearing outperformed the other two groups significantly (P˂0.001) across all indicators in both tasks. Additionally, cochlear-implanted children exhibited better performance than those with hearing aids. Children with hearing aids demonstrated a significant difference in all phonological indices in the picture naming and description tasks. A significant difference was observed only in the PMLU and PVC indices in the cochlear implantation group. In contrast, differences were noted in the PMLU and PWC indices in the normal hearing group.
As mentioned, the results showed that children with hearing impairment performed worse on all phonological indices compared to their normal peers. These results were consistent with those of Schauwers et al. who found that children with normal hearing exhibited superior performance in whole-word indices compared to cochlear-implanted children [26]. 
The current study’s results are also consistent with the results of Baudnock et al. who demonstrated that children with CI and hearing aids tended to make more phonological errors and employ more phonological processes in their speech than children with normal hearing [31]. Additionally, Zanichelli revealed that children with normal hearing performed better in the PCC index than their counterparts with hearing aids [25]. The increased occurrence of consonant errors in hearing-impaired children with hearing aids and CI compared to those with normal hearing can be attributed to auditory sense deprivation, as children rely on auditory experiences to produce correct consonants [32]. Hearing loss can affect the connectivity of certain parts of the central auditory system and interfere with the development of the central auditory system, which plays a vital role in cognitive and language processing, especially phonological development. Therefore, the absence of hearing influences cognitive development. Studies have emphasized that early exposure to sound is vital for the typical development of cognitive abilities, including working memory associated with phonological development [31]. 
However, contrary to the results of the present study, Faes et al. found no significant difference in the phonological whole-word indices between the two groups of CI and normal-hearing children. This longitudinal study found a significant difference in the phonological whole word indices at the beginning of cochlear implantation. However, it disappeared and was eventually compensated for older ages. The results of their research are consistent with the conclusion that an increase in syllable length affects the phonological whole-word performance of children, and this is more effective in cochlear-implanted children [14].
Low and So showed a difference in the PCC index between the two groups of children with hearing aids and CI [32]. These researchers believed that CI are more effective than hearing aids in improving the phonological skills of hearing-impaired children, which is confirmed by the current research and other studies [33].
As mentioned above, this study’s PCC and PVC indices differed significantly among the three groups. The group with normal hearing had better performance than the other two groups, and the mean scores of the CI group were significantly higher than those of children with hearing aids. These results were consistent with Sadat Seyedi et al.’s regarding the difference between children with normal hearing in the PCC index and the two groups of hearing-impaired children. However, the results did not match the difference between children with CI and hearing aids. Despite our results, the PVC index was almost the same in the three groups in the study by Sadat Seyedi et al. [34]. These differences may be due to the different characteristics of the participants in the two studies. The participants in Seyedi’s study were only 6-year-old children, while in the present study, the age range of the children was 3-6 years [34].
In Yi ’s research, the average PCC index was 75% in children with CI and 74% in children with hearing aids, which differs from our results. This difference may be due to the different age groups and input criteria, including the degree of hearing loss [23].
Co-articulation is influenced by the context of a word and adjacent sounds [35]. The speech units are not separate from each other in the word and the sentence, and in conversational speech, it is clear that the phonological properties of words affect the words before and after them [36, 37]. As a result, people do not perform similarly when evaluating picture naming and description. 
In the present study, children with hearing aids revealed a significant difference in all phonological indices in picture naming and description. Since no similar research findings were observed, the children’s better performance in PMLU, PCC, and PVC indices in picture description compared to picture naming might be related to the grammatical features of the Persian language. Using the grammatical features of the Persian language changes the syllabic context of words, and the increase or decrease of syllable length and context changes directly affect these indices.
Faes et al. have shown that children with CI differ in phonemic accuracy from children with normal hearing, not by their hearing status alone, but by the interaction between their hearing status and the complexity and length of target words. The production of longer and more complex words differentiates the two groups, not the production of short and simple words [14]. Faes et al. stated that in children with normal hearing or CI, it is unclear how the context of speech sampling affects phonemic accuracy, and that further research is needed on this [22].
The relationship between grammatical and phonological features has been the research subject, with various studies conducted in this field. Some linguists believe that the relationship between a language’s grammatical and phonological areas is related to each language’s specific characteristics. All these cases have in common that the addition or subtraction of suffixes and grammatical prefixes affects the type and texture of words, and the type of effect changes depending on the texture and class of the word [38, 39]. However, the relationship between grammatical features and phonological indicators must be further investigated in children’s speech. In addition, these results are consistent with the research of Willis, Faes et al. who showed that an increase in syllable length decreases production accuracy [14, 23, 40]. For example, when the child uses the object /ra/ as an abbreviation in the sentence /pesār chātro bārdāʃt/, the syllabic texture of /chātr/ (umbrella) changes from monosyllabic to bisyllabic, and the word with a consonant cluster becomes a word without a cluster. The CVCC texture changes to two CVC-CV syllables.
Also, the results showed a significant relationship between age and hearing ability with all phonological indices except PVC. Consistent with the study’s results, Ingram [16] and Taelman et al. [24] showed that the amount of PMLU increases with age. Zanichelli found that the PCC index was greater in children with normal hearing than those with hearing loss [25]. Schauwers et al. concluded that the performance of Dutch children with CI is weaker than that of their peers with normal hearing in phonological indices [26]. Also, Baudonck et al. investigated phonological processes and determined that children with hearing aids have more phonological errors than those with CI [31]. This study had limitations, including the lack of access to the exact age of hearing loss and the extended sampling time due to the spread of the coronavirus pandemic and the closure of the centers during the peak times of the coronavirus. Based on the results and limitations of this study, the following suggestions are made for future research:
1) The role of morphological-syntactic features in the phonological indicators of the whole word should be investigated; 2) Despite the research team’s great efforts to control confounding factors, it is suggested that factors, such as the exact time of hearing loss diagnosis, the use of a prosthesis, and the start and number of sessions and type of treatments should be controlled. 3) To determine the range of PMLU, PWP, and PWC indices in normal Persian-speaking children in longitudinal studies. 4) To investigate and research whole word phonological indicators in other children with speech sound disorders and bilingual children.

Conclusion
This study showed a significant difference in the three groups studied, the phonological indices of the whole word PMLU, PWP, PWC, PCC, and PVC indices. Children with normal hearing had better performance than cochlear-implanted and hearing-impaired children, and cochlear-implanted children also had better performance than children with hearing aids. The results of the present study showed that the phonological characteristics of hearing-impaired children are deficient due to the lack of a sense of hearing and require a detailed speech therapy program. Using a CI has a greater effect on improving a hearing-impaired child’s performance in phonology. Also, whole word indices, such as PMLU, which relate to word complexity and length, better show phonological features in the two tasks of picture naming and picture description. Therefore, in the phonological assessment and treatment of hearing-impaired children, in addition to focusing on different parts of the word (consonants and vowels), we should not only focus on the different parts of the word (consonants and vowels), but also on the phonetic indicators of the whole word and the selection of the target vocabulary.

Ethical Considerations

Compliance with ethical guidelines

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran (Code: IR.MUMS.REC.1399.615). The research project was explained to the parents of children who met the inclusion criteria, and after signing the consent form, their child entered the study.

Funding
This study was extracted from the master's thesis of Haniyeh Jafarzadeh, approved by the Faculty of Paramedical Sciences and Rehabilitation, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran. This research was supported by a research project, funded by Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran (No.: 991248).

Authors' contributions
Conceptualization and study design: Fatemeh Haresabadi; Data collection: Haniyeh Jafarzadeh, Fatemeh Haresabadi, and Majid Haddadi Avval; Data analysis: Zahra Abbasi Shaye; Writing the original draft: Toktam Maleki Shahmahmood, Haniyeh Jafarzadeh, and Fatemeh Haresabadi; Review and editing: Zahra Ghayoumi-Anaraki and Toktam Maleki Shahmahmood; Final approval: All authors.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
The authors thank all collaborators of the Mashhad Naghmeh and Parvaneha Centre, Kindergartens, and children’s parents.




References
  1. Schwartz RG. Handbook of child language disorders. New York: Psychology Press; 2010. [DOI:10.4324/9780203837764]
  2. Sundarrajan M, Tobey EA, Nicholas J, Geers AE. Assessing consonant production in children with cochlear implants. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2019; 84:105966. [DOI:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.105966] [PMID]
  3. Olusanya BO, Davis AC, Hoffman HJ. Hearing loss: Rising prevalence and impact. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2019; 97(10):646-646A. [DOI:10.2471/BLT.19.224683] [PMID] 
  4. Moeller MP, Tomblin JB, Yoshinaga-Itano C, Connor CM, Jerger S. Current state of knowledge: Language and literacy of children with hearing impairment. Ear and Hearing. 2007; 28(6):740-53. [DOI:10.1097/AUD.0b013e318157f07f] [PMID]
  5. Kent RD, Bauer HR. Vocalizations of one-year-olds. Journal of Child Language. 1985; 12(3):491-526. [DOI:10.1017/S0305000900006620]
  6. Smit AB, Hand L, Freilinger JJ, Bernthal JE, Bird A. The Iowa articulation norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1990; 55(4):779-98. [DOI:10.1044/jshd.5504.779] [PMID]
  7. Stoel-Gammon C, Dunn C. Normal and disordered phonology in children. Austin: Pro-Ed, Inc; 1985. [Link]
  8. Hodson BW, Paden EP. Phonological processes which characterize unintelligible and intelligible speech in early childhood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1981; 46(4):369-73. [DOI:10.1044/jshd.4604.369]
  9. Watson MM, Terrell P. Longitudinal changes in phonological whole-word measures in 2-year-olds. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2012; 14(4):351-62. [DOI:10.3109/17549507.2012.663936] [PMID]
  10. Naidoo Y, Van der Merwe A, Groenewald E, Naudé E. Development of speech sounds and syllable structure of words in Zulu-speaking children. Southern African Linguistics and Applied Language Studies. 2005; 23(1):59-79. [DOI:10.2989/16073610509486374]
  11. Habib MG, Waltzman SB, Tajudeen B, Svirsky MA. Speech production intelligibility of early implanted pediatric cochlear implant users. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 2010; 74(8):855-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijporl.2010.04.009] [PMID] 
  12. Shriberg LD, Austin D, Lewis BA, McSweeny JL, Wilson DL. The percentage of consonants correct (PCC) metric: Extensions and reliability data. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1997; 40(4):708-22. [DOI:10.1044/jslhr.4004.708] [PMID]
  13. Ingram D, Ingram KD. A whole-word approach to phonological analysis and intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2001; 32(4):271-83. [DOI:10.1044/0161-1461(2001/024)] [PMID]
  14. Faes J, Gillis J, Gillis S. Phonemic accuracy development in children with cochlear implants up to five years of age by using Levenshtein distance. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2016; 59:40-58. [DOI:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.09.004] [PMID]
  15. Babatsouli E, Ingram D, Sotiropoulos D. Phonological word proximity in child speech development. Chaotic Modeling and Simulation. 2014; 4(3):295-313. [Link]
  16. Ingram D. The measurement of whole-word productions. Journal of Child Language. 2002; 29(4):713-33. [DOI:10.1017/s0305000902005275] [PMID]
  17. McCabe RB, Bradley DP. Pre-and Postarticulation Therapy Assessment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 1973; 4(1):13-22. [DOI:10.1044/0161-1461.0401.13]
  18. Beers M, Rodenburg-Van Wee M, Gerrits E. Whole-word measures and the speech production of typically developing Dutch children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2019; 33(12):1149-64. [DOI:10.1080/02699206.2019.1596163] [PMID]
  19. Bunta F, Fabiano‐Smith L, Goldstein B, Ingram D. Phonological whole‐word measures in 3‐year‐old bilingual children and their age‐matched monolingual peers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2009; 23(2):156-75. [DOI:10.1080/02699200802603058] [PMID]
  20. MACLeod AA, Laukys K, Rvachew S. The impact of bilingual language learning on whole-word complexity and segmental accuracy among children aged 18 and 36 months. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2011; 13(6):490-9. [DOI:10.3109/17549507.2011.578658] [PMID]
  21. van Noort-van der Spek IL, Franken MC, Wieringa MH, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Phonological development in very-low-birthweight children: An exploratory study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2010; 52(6):541-6. [DOI:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03507.x] [PMID]
  22. Faes J. Speech production and speech production accuracy in young children: Hearing and hearing impaired children with a cochlear implant [PhD dissertation]. Antwerp: University of Antwerp; 2017. [Link]
  23. Yi H. Speech production outcomes in young children with early identified hearing loss [PhD dissertation]. The University of Texas at Austin; 2018. [Link]
  24. Taelman H, Durieux G, Gillis S. Notes on Ingram’s whole-word measures for phonological development. Journal of Child Language. 2005; 32(2):391-405. [DOI:10.1017/s0305000904006774] [PMID]
  25. Zanichelli L, Gil D. [Percentage of Consonants Correct (PCC) in children with and without hearing loss (Portuguese)]. Jornal da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia. 2011; 23(2):107-13. [DOI:10.1590/s2179-64912011000200005] [PMID]
  26. Schauwers K, Taelman H, Gillis S, Govaerts P. Phonological proficiency and accuracy of young hearing-impaired children with a cochlear implant. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing ; 2008. [Link]
  27. Sevinc S, Ozcebe E, Atas A, Buyukozturk S. Articulation skills in Turkish speaking children with cochlear implant. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 2009; 73(10):1430-3. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijporl.2009.07.011] [PMID]
  28. Kunnari S, Saaristo-Helin K, Savinainen-Makkonen T. Phonological mean length of utterance in specific language impairment: A multi-case study of children acquiring Finnish. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2012; 26(5):428-44. [DOI:10.3109/02699206.2012.655840] [PMID]
  29. Zarifian T, Modarresi Y, Gholami Tehrani L, Dastjerdi Kazemi M, Salavati M. The Persian version of phonological test of diagnostic evaluation articulation and phonology for Persian speaking children and investigating its validity and reliability. Auditory and Vestibular Research. 2014; 23(4):10-20. [Link]
  30. Alexander P, Chang CM, Yang CH, Alkhateeb H, Oaks J. Publications by University of Indianapolis Faculty and Staff. Historia Mathematica. 2005; 32:400-25.
  31. Baudonck N, Dhooge I, D'haeseleer E, Van Lierde K. A comparison of the consonant production between Dutch children using cochlear implants and children using hearing aids. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 2010; 74(4):416-21. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijporl.2010.01.017] [PMID]
  32. Law ZW, So LK. Phonological abilities of hearing-impaired Cantonese-speaking children with cochlear implants or hearing AIDS. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006; 49(6):1342-53. [DOI:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/096)] [PMID]
  33. Geers AE, Tobey E. Effects of cochlear implants and tactile aids on the development of speech production skills in children with profound hearing impairment. The Volta Review. 1992; 94(5):135–63. [Link]
  34. Sadat Sayedi E, Mehri A, Maroufizadeh S, Zarifian T. [A study of phonological disorders in six-year-old children with cochlear implants and hearing Aids as compared to normal hearing children in Tehran, Iran (Persian)]. Journal of Mazandaran University of Medical Sciences. 2017; 27(147):398-403. [Link]
  35. Ball MJ, Perkins MR, Müller N, Howard S. The handbook of clinical linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers; 2008. [DOI:10.1002/9781444301007]
  36. Bleile KM. The manual of speech sound disorders: A book for students and clinicians. Stamford, CT: Cengage Learning; 2015. [Link]
  37. Ohala JJ. Coarticulation and phonology. Language and Speech. 1993; 36(2-3):155-70. [DOI:10.1177/002383099303600303] [PMID]
  38. Bermúdez-Otero R, Honeybone P. Phonology and syntax: A shifting relationship. Lingua. 2006; 116(5):543-61. [Link]
  39. Klavans JL. The independence of syntax and phonology in cliticization. Language. 1985; 61(1):95-120. [DOI:10.2307/413422]
  40. Gathercole SE, Willis C, Emslie H, Baddeley AD. The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children’s repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1991; 12(3):349-67. [DOI:10.1017/S0142716400009267]
Article type: Original Research Articles | Subject: Speech therapy
Received: 2023/10/8 | Accepted: 2024/02/17 | Published: 2025/06/1

Send email to the article author


Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb