Introduction
Visual perception and work memory are foundational cognitive functions that play a central role in daily activities. Visual perception is a neuropsychological process involving the interpretation and integration of sensory information at the cortical level [
1]. This complex process enables individuals to recognize and identify attributes of objects, such as shapes, colors, and spatial relationships [
2, 3].
Research suggests that visual perception is not merely a passive reception of stimuli but actively shapes how individuals understand and interact with their surroundings. Perceptual skills, such as visual attention, discrimination and memory, are vital for effective learning and comprehension [
1,
4].
Neurophysiological studies indicate heightened activity in brain regions, like the occipital cortex during visual tasks, underscoring the neural basis of visual information processing and retention [
4-
6].
Working memory, introduced by A. Baddeley and Hitch [
7], is a complex cognitive system comprising multiple components that can be selectively influenced [
8, 9]. Functional neuroimaging studies have revealed that fronto-parietal networks are activated during verbal and visuo-spatial working memory tasks [
10]. Baddeley defines working memory as a system of limited capacity, which allows for the temporary storage and manipulation of information that is necessary for the performance of complex cognitive tasks, such as comprehension, learning, and reasoning [
2].
The interplay between visual perception and memory is particularly noteworthy. Effective visual processing enhances memory retention while working memory influences how visual information is interpreted and utilized [
5,
11]. Research shows that difficulties in visual perception often manifest as challenges in reading, copying, writing and math [
12, 13]. Similarly, working memory abilities have been linked to various learning situations, such as vocabulary acquisition and mental arithmetic [
10].
Despite the critical role of these cognitive functions in learning, relatively few studies have explored the correlations between visual perception, working memory and academic performance, particularly during adolescence. This gap is especially evident in the context of middle school students with learning difficulties. To address this, the current study examines the relationship between visual perception, working memory, and academic performance among middle school students in Morocco’s Middle Atlas region who experience learning difficulties. Understanding these connections can provide valuable insights into cognitive processes underlying academic success and inform strategies to support students facing learning challenges.
Materials and Methods
Study design and setting
This study employed a cross-sectional design to investigate the relationship between visual perception, working memory, and academic performance among adolescents. The study involved 122 college students from three colleges located in the Province of Khenifra, situated in the Middle Atlas Mountains of Morocco. The participants had a MEAN±SD age of 14.1±1.4 years, ranging from a minimum age of 12 years to a maximum of 18 years.
Study procedure
Neuropsychological assessment tool
In this study, the Rey–Osterrieth complex figure test (ROCF), also known as ROCF-A (
Figure 1), was employed.
This neuropsychological test evaluates multiple cognitive functions, including visuospatial and visuo-constructive abilities, non-verbal and working memory, attention, and planning [
14]. To assess visual perception and working memory, a numerical version of ROCF-A was used, comprising the following elements: Raster paper which is a surface with a pattern of small dots that appears as a simple grey background, minimizing distractions for the user; digital pen (Anoto DP-201 Pen) which is a precision tool equipped with an infrared laser camera, capable of accurately tracking movements on the raster paper (
Figure 2); Elian software (Expert Line Information ANalyser) facilitates visualization and analysis of input data and provides near-instantaneous feedback, along with an easy-to-use Excel-compatible data table for statistical evaluation.
The combination of Elian software (Expert version), and the digital pen allows for the collection of highly precise, objective data, with an accuracy of 1/10 of a mm and 1/100 of 1 s. This setup ensures reliable measurement of visual perception and working memory performance.
Selection of study groups
To examine the relationship between visual perception, working memory, and academic performance among the adolescents in our sample, we recruited two distinct groups as shown in
Figure 3.
Group 1 (learners with learning difficulties; n=59) was identified based on an in-depth review of the learners’ academic records, their academic performance, and input from their teachers. Meanwhile, group 2 (control group; n=63) consisted of learners without learning difficulties, serving as a comparison group.
Procedure and scoring of the ROCF test
Passing the test
The test was administered in two stages. The copying stage: The model is placed horizontally in front of the participant, ensuring clear visibility. The participant is instructed to copy the figure as accurately as possible. Reproduction stage: After the model is removed, the participant is asked to reproduce the figure from memory. Wallon and Mesmin recommend a waiting period of less than 3 min between these stages. The time allowed for each stage is unrestricted [
14].
Scoring methods
André Rey developed two scoring approaches to evaluate participants’ performance on the test.
Numerical scoring: The ROCF-A is divided into 18 elements (
Figure 1) and each element is scored as follows:
1) Unrecognizable or absent (0 points); 2) Deformed or incomplete but recognizable and poorly placed (1 point); 3) Deformed or incomplete but recognizable and well placed, or correctly drawn but poorly placed (2 points); 4) Correctly drawn, well placed but imperfect (3 points); 5) Correctly drawn and well placed (4 points). The total score ranges from 0 to 72, with higher scores indicating better performance [
14].
Type rating: The type rating method evaluates the strategy used by the participant to copy and reproduce the figure. The ELIAN software, with its " expert" version, analyzes the sequence of strokes to identify the organizational strategy employed. Wallon and Mesmin categorized seven organizational strategies.
Type I (construction of the framework): The participant begins by drawing the large central rectangle, which serves as a framework to group all other elements. This strategy is the most structured and advanced. Type II (details included in the framework): The participant starts with a detail (e.g. the upper left cross or a diagonal) and then completes the central rectangle, as in Type I. Type III (general outline): The participant reproduces the full outline of the figure, without differentiating the central rectangle. Interior details are added to this container. Type IV (juxtaposition of details): The participant reproduces the figure by adding details one by one, like assembling a puzzle, without a guiding framework. This approach often results in distortion. Type V (details on a confusing background): The participant produces a poorly organized graphic where the overall pattern unrecognizable, though some individual details are may be identifiable. Type VI (reduction to a familiar pattern): The participant simplifies the figure into a familiar shape (e.g. a house, boat, or fish) that vaguely resembles the original model. Type VII (scribble): The participant produces a scribble with no recognizable elements or structure. This dual-scoring method provides a comprehensive analysis of both the accuracy of the figure reproduction and the cognitive strategies used. It enables an in-depth understanding of participants’ visuospatial organization, planning abilities, and memory processes.
Statistical analysis
The collected data were initially organized and filtered in Excel before being transferred to SPSS software, version 25 for statistical analysis. Both descriptive and inferential statistical methods were utilized in this study. Descriptive statistics, including measures such as median and Mean±SD, were used to summarize the central tendencies and variability within the data. For analytical purposes, the Pearson correlation, independent sample t-test and multiple linear regression were applied to explore relationships between variables. A P=0.05 was set as the threshold for statistical significance.
Results
Socio-demographic and educational profile of the sample
During the study period, 122 learners agreed to participate in this study with an average age of 14.1±1.4 years. Regarding gender, we noted a sex ratio of 1/2 with a non-significant differentiation between the two sexes (chi-squared=1.181 and P>0.05). For the environment, 53% of the participants were from rural areas while 47% of the participants were from urban areas with a non-significant difference between the two sexes (chi-squared=0.295 and P>0.05). For school repetition, 31% of the adolescents in our population have repeated at least one school year with an average of 1.2 years, while 69% of the participants have never repeated a year with a statistically significant difference between the two modalities (chi-squared=17.344 and P<0.001;
Table 1).
Studying the relationship between visual perception, work memory, and academic performance
Visual perception and academic performance: the copying phase of the ROCF-A
The numerical quotation during the copy phase included comparison of the means of accuracy scores and errors-omissions in the copying phase of the Rey complex figure between learners in the learning-disabled group and the control group through independent sample t-tests of the results are shown in
Table 2.

According to the results of the copying phase of the ROCF-A, the group with learning difficulties had a low mean accuracy score of 59.98±11.34 compared to the control group without learning difficulties with the following mean: 66.68±3.72 and the independent sample t-test shows a statistically significant mean difference between the two groups regarding accuracy scores in the copying phase of the ROCF-A (t=4.44, P˂0.001,
Table 2). Similarly, the group with learning difficulties had a higher mean error/omission score of 12.01±11.34 compared to the control group without learning difficulties with the following mean: 5.31±3.72 and, the independent sample t-test showed a statistically significant difference between the two groups regarding accuracy scores in the copying phase of the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test (ROCF-A) (t=-4.44, P˂0.001,
Table 2).
The quotation in types during the copying phase
The analysis of the type of achievement used by the learners in the two groups leads to the results presented in
Figure 4.

Comparison of the type of achievement during the copying phase shows significant differences between learners in the two groups: 31.74% of learners in the control group achieved Type I (framework construction) compared to only 11.68% of learners with learning difficulties. Type II (details included in the framework) was completed by 38.98% of learners in the control group versus 27.11% of learners in the difficulty group. Type III (general outline) was completed by almost the same number of learners in both groups. For Type IV (the juxtaposition of details), 52.54% of the learners with difficulties performed this type of exercise compared to 25.42% of the learners in the control group. Type VI (reduction to a familiar pattern) was completed by only one learner with learning difficulties. In the same way, the chi-squared test allows us to verify the existence of a significant link between the different types during the copying phase of the ROCF-A and academic performance with a chi-squared value of 14.076 (P<0.05;
Figure 4).
Investigating the relationship between visual perception and academic performance: The reproduction phase of the ROCF-A
The numerical quotation during the reproduction phase
Comparison of the means of the accuracy scores and the errors-omissions in the memory phase of the complex figure of Rey between the learners of the group in the difficulty of learning and the control group through independent sample t-test shows the results presented in
Table 3.

Based on the results of the reproduction or memory phase of the ROCF-A, the learning-disabled group had a low mean accuracy score of 33.79±14.20 compared to the control group with a mean of 46.11±12.89 and the independent sample t-test shows a statistically significant difference between the two groups regarding accuracy scores in the copying phase of the ROCF-A (t=5.01, P˂0.001). Similarly, the learning-disabled group had a higher mean error and omission score of 38.20±12.89 compared to the control group, which had fewer errors with a mean of 25.88±14.20 and the independent sample t-test shows a statistically significant difference between the two groups regarding accuracy scores in the memory phase of the ROCF-A (t=-5.01, P˂0.001,
Table 3).
The quotation in types during the reproduction phase
Analysis of the type of achievement used by learners in both groups yields the results shown in
Figure 5.

Comparison of the type of achievement during the replication phase in memory shows significant differences between learners in the two groups: 19.04% of learners in the control group achieved type I (frame construction) compared to only 11.86% of learners with learning difficulties. Type II (details included in the framework) was completed by 52.38% of learners in the control group versus 42.37% of learners in the difficulty group. Type III (general outline) was done only by learners with difficulties. For type IV (the juxtaposition of details), 33.89% of the learners with difficulties performed this type compared to 28.57% of the learners in the control group. Type VI (reduction to a familiar pattern), like type III, was completed by only two learners with learning difficulties. In the same way, the chi-squared test allows us to verify the existence of a significant link between the different types during the phase of reproduction in memory of the ROCF-A and school performance with a value of chi-squared=9.403 and P<0.05 (
Figure 5).
The multiple correspondence analysis of the studied variables (age, environment, copy score, copy types, reproduction score, reproduction types, academic performance, repetition) showed a homogeneity between the modalities at the level of the two dimensions 1 and 2 with a Cronbach α of 0.79 and 0.60, respectively (
Table 4).

The projection of these modalities in the space bounded by axes I and II, which alone absorb 72% of the total variation, defines two major groups (
Figure 6).
The first group is characterized by the learners in the control group, aged between 12 and 15 years old living in an urban environment, not repeating a year, the best performers in copy (visual perception) and reproduction (working memory) accuracy scores, who carry out the most methodical execution schemes (type I and II).
The second group is categorically opposed to this first group. It is characterized by learners with learning difficulties with a high repetition rate, aged between 16 and 18 years old, living in rural areas, the least performing in terms of accuracy score in the copying phase (visual perception) and in the reproduction phase (working memory) who perform the least methodical execution patterns (type III and VI).
Discussion
The use of the numerical version of the complex figure of Rey -A (ROCF-A), during this study, aimed to highlight the relationship between two cognitive functions that are solicited by the two phases of the ROCF-A test: Visual perception and working memory on the one hand and academic performance on the other hand in a sample of college students. The results of this study showed that learners with learning difficulties recorded lower accuracy scores, committed more errors, had more omissions, and performed the least methodical patterns of execution compared to the control group and those in the copying and reproduction in memory phases of the ROCF-A test. These results make it possible to establish a link between learning difficulties and learners’ performance in cognitive tasks involving visual perception and working memory. The conclusions of this study are in line with previous work on visual perception, a significant association between reading and visual discrimination has been described, a significant association with poor visual and motor integration (low visual-motor) has been observed in 22% of dysgraphia and dyscalculia learners and a correlation between written skills and visual-motor abilities has been reported [
12]. The finding that children with learning difficulties in mathematics had significantly more visual perception problems compared to control children was consistent with some studies, but this contrasts with the findings of other authors in a more heterogeneous sample of participants between 6 and 19 years of age [
13]. Other researchers [
15] have linked math problems with visual perception problems and or motor skills as well as the integration of the two. According to other researchers visual processing of time was an important factor in several language skills such as spelling and word reading [
16]. Other investigations have shown a strong association between reading and spelling performance and different categories of perception including visual perception [
17]. Regarding working memory, learners with learning difficulties show poor performance in visuospatial MDT compared to learners in the control group. These findings are consistent with other research that has established the relationship between learners’ performance on tasks that require visual working memory and their achievement in English, math, and science [
18]. Other studies have shown specific associations between performance in visuospatial working memory tasks and low math achievement [
19], and similarly investigations have found a positive correlation between learners’ math and reading performance and working memory ability [
20]. Researchers have suggested that the three components proposed by Baddeley may play a role in mental arithmetic [
21]. Similarly, the involvement of working memory in a wide variety of complex cognitive activities, such as reasoning, comprehension and problem solving has been demonstrated by several studies making it a major determinant of academic success [
5,
22-
37].
Conclusion
This study emphasizes that learners with learning difficulties consistently exhibit poor performance on tasks requiring visual perception and working memory. These two cognitive functions play a critical role in a wide range of learning activities, including reading, writing, mathematics, and problem-solving. Impairments in these areas not only hinder academic achievement but can also impact self-esteem, motivation, and overall cognitive development.
Given the fundamental importance of visual perception and working memory in educational success, we underscore the necessity of comprehensive neurocognitive evaluations and multidisciplinary diagnostic approaches. Such assessments can help identify learners who exhibit deficits in these areas at an early stage, enabling timely and targeted interventions. A neurocognitive evaluation should include tests that assess not only visual perception and working memory but also related skills such as visual-motor integration, attention, and executive functioning.
Moreover, the integration of a multidisciplinary team comprising educators, psychologists, neuropsychologists, and speech or occupational therapists is vital to developing individualized support plans. These plans can include cognitive training, remediation programs and classroom accommodations tailored to the specific needs of learners. For instance, interventions such as visual-perceptual skill training, working memory enhancement exercises, and assistive technology tools can provide practical support for students struggling in these areas.
In addition to individualized interventions, broader systemic efforts are needed to address these challenges. Schools and educational institutions should prioritize teacher training to recognize signs of cognitive difficulties, implement evidence-based strategies for remediation, and foster an inclusive learning environment. Policymakers and educators must also ensure that resources and tools for neurocognitive assessment and support are accessible, particularly in underserved regions or populations where such difficulties may go unnoticed.
Future research should explore the effectiveness of various neurocognitive interventions and their long-term impact on learners’ academic performance and overall development. Longitudinal studies with larger and more diverse samples could provide deeper insights into the complex relationships between cognitive functions and learning outcomes. Expanding this research would not only enhance our understanding of these deficits but also contribute to the development of innovative solutions to support learners with learning difficulties.
Identifying and addressing deficits in visual perception and working memory is crucial for enabling learners to reach their full academic potential. By adopting a proactive, multidisciplinary approach to assessment and intervention, educators and clinicians can help mitigate the challenges faced by these learners, fostering improved educational outcomes and long-term success.
Study limitations
This study faced several limitations that warrant consideration. First, the sample size of 122 participants, while adequate for initial analysis, may restrict the generalizability of the findings to a broader population. A larger sample size would enhance the statistical power and allow for more robust conclusions about the relationships between visual perception, working memory, and academic performance. Second, the division of participants into groups based on academic performance introduces potential biases. These biases may stem from unmeasured factors such as self-esteem, motivation, socio-economic background, and other psychosocial variables that were not controlled for in this study. Third, without matching participants on certain, it is difficult to determine the extent to which these factors influenced the observed outcomes. Finally, while the methods used, including the ROCF-A test and its digital adaptations, provide valuable insights, they do not account for potential cultural or contextual differences that may influence task performance. For example, students’ familiarity with digital tools or specific test formats may vary, potentially affecting their results.
Future research should aim to address these limitations by employing a longitudinal design to track changes over time. Expanding the sample to include a more diverse population in terms of socioeconomic background, geographic location, and academic achievement would improve the generalizability of the findings. Moreover, controlling for psychosocial and contextual factors through matching or advanced statistical modeling could provide a clearer understanding of the direct and indirect relationships between visual perception, working memory and academic performance. Finally, exploring additional cognitive and environmental variables would offer a more comprehensive perspective on the complex interplay of factors influencing learning outcomes.
Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines
This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Sidi Mohamed Ben Abdallah University, Fez, Morocco (FLSH04/2024). This research was conducted with utmost commitment to ethical considerations, ensuring the voluntary collaboration of participants, upholding patients’ right to self-determination and rigorously adhering to ethical guidelines regarding privacy, anonymity, and data confidentiality.
Funding
This research has been supported by “Le Centre National de Recherche Scientifique et Technique, Morocco: Cov/2020/28)” and by the “Lifelong Learning Observatory (UNESCO), Sidi Mohamed Ben Abdalah University, Fez, Morocco.
Authors' contributions
Conceptualization: Mounir Bouzaboul; Methodology: Francesco Chirico, Zakaria Abidli, Driss Ait Ali, Murat Yildirim and Amelia Rizzo; Software: Zakaria Abidli, Younes Rami, Amèle El Achhab, Ziri Rabea and Łukasz Szarpak; Validation: Francesco Chirico, Amelia Rizzo, Livio Tarchi and Kavita Batra; Formal Analysis: Zakaria Abidli; Investigation: Zakaria Abidli, Mounir Bouzaboul and Abdeslam Amri; Resources: Amèle EL Achhab, Ziri Rabea; Data collection: Zakaria Abidli, Abdeslam Amri and Adil Hadri; Writing the original draft: Mounir Bouzaboul; Supervision: Francesco Chiricoand Amelia Rizzo; Project administration: Hicham Khabbache; Funding acquisition: Hicham Khabbache; Review and editing: All authors.
Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors extend their sincere gratitude to all the individuals who participated in the study.
References
- Eloirdi A, Mammad K, Arfaoui A, Chtibi H, Diallo MC, Wallon P, et al. Study of visual perception and working memory in Moroccan adolescents attending school. Open Journal of Medical Psychology. 2018; 7(4):82-90. [Link]
- Schneck CM. Visual perception. In: Case-Smith J, Allen AS, Pratt PN, editors. Occupational therapy for children. 3th ed. Michigan: Mosby; 1996. [Link]
- Khabbache H, Ouazizi K, Bragazzi NL, Watfa AA, Mrabet R. Cognitive development: Towards a pluralistic and coalitional model. Cosmos and History. 2020; 16(2):245-65. [Link]
- Maksimenko VA, Runnova AE, Zhuravlev MO, Makarov V V, Nedayvozov V, Grubov VV, et al. Visual perception affected by motivation and alertness controlled by a noninvasive brain-computer interface. PLoS One 2017; 12(12):e0188700. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0188700] [PMID]
- Corbin L, Moissenet A, Camos V. [Working memory functioning in children with learning difficulties (French)]. Développements 2012; 11(2):5-12. [DOI:10.3917/devel.011.0005]
- Agam Y, Sekuler R. Interactions between working memory and visual perception: An ERP/EEG study. Neuroimage. 2007; 36(3):933-42. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.014] [PMID]
- Cowan N. Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. Psychological Bulletin. 1988; 104(2):163-91. [Link]
- Majerus S. [The role of working memory in learning and its disorders (French)]. ANAE: Approche Neuropsychologique des Apprentissages chez l'Enfant. 2017; 149:415-21. [Link]
- Fassima A, Ait Ali D, Khabbache H. Improving working memory performance in healthy older adults: Investigating the training effects on central executive through a quasi-experimental approach. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2025; 2(1):27-34. [Link]
- Kosma A. [The specific functioning of working memory in translation (French)]. Meta. 2007; 52(1):22-8. [DOI:10.7202/014716ar]
- Khabbache H, Ali DA. Neuroplasticity and cognitive development: Interdisciplinary perspectives on psychotherapeutic and educational approaches. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2024; 2(1):1-4. [Link]
- Mona G, Dhadwad V, Yeradkar R, Adhikari A, Setia M, Therapy Dept O, et al. Study of visual perceptual problems in children with learning disability. Indian Journal of Basic and Applied Medical Research. 2015; 4(3):492-7. [Link]
- Vaz-Cerniglia C, Anaut M, Portalier S. Specificity of the rey complex figure in children with attention disorders. Bulletin de Psychologie. 2005; 4(478):435-45. [DOI:10.3917/bupsy.478.0435]
- Wallon, P, Mesmin C. [Rey complex figure A and B: Guide to use and interpretation (French)]. London: Pearson; 2009.
- Sortor JM, Kulp MT. Are the results of the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration and its subtests related to achievement test scores? Optometry and vision science. 2003; 80(11):758-63. [DOI:10.1097/00006324-200311000-00013] [PMID]
- Zhang S, Xia X, Li F, Chen C, Zhao L. Study on visual and auditory perception characteristics of children with different type of mathematics learning disability. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education. 2021; 68(1):78-94. [DOI:10.1080/1034912X.2019.1634248]
- Dhingra R, Manhas S, Kohli N. Relationship of perceptual abilities with academic performance of children. Journal of Social Sciences. 2010; 23(2):143-7. [Link]
- St Clair-Thompson HL, Gathercole SE. Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. 59(4):745-59. [PMID]
- Mammarella IC, Pazzaglia F. Visual perception and memory impairments in children at risk of nonverbal learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychology. 2010; 16(6):564-76. [DOI:10.1080/09297049.2010.485125] [PMID]
- Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and individual differences. 2011; 21(4):327-36. [DOI:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007] [PMID]
- Chan BM, Ho CS. The cognitive profile of Chinese children with mathematics difficulties. Journal of experimental Child Psychology. 2010; 107(3):260-79. [DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.016] [PMID]
- Ait Ben Ali S, Korchyou Y, Ait Baja Z, Khiri F. Metacognitive learning strategies and academic performance: A correlational study among Moroccan nursing students. Advances in Medicine, Psychology and Public Health. 2024; 1(3):125-32. [Link]
- Chirico F, Rizzo A. Tackling mental health disorders, burnout, workplace violence, post-traumatic stress disorders amidst climate change, and new global challenges: The crucial role of emotional management education. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2024; 2(1):5-7. [Link]
- Hilal M, Khabbache H, Ali DA. Dropping out of school: A psychosocial approach. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2024; 1(1):26-36. [Link]
- Tarchi L, Castellini G, Ricca V. Zipf's law: Divergence from general content in online communities of peers affected by attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and anorexia nervosa. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2024; 1(2):82-91. [DOI:10.5281/zenodo.106374]
- Rizzo A, Sorrenti L, Commendatore M, Mautone A, Caparello C, Maggio MG, et al. Caregivers of children with Autism spectrum disorders: The role of guilt sensitivity and support. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2024; 13(14):4249. [DOI:10.3390/jcm13144249] [PMID]
- Tusha A, Bulut S, Al-Hendawi M. Promoting a healthy school environment via social-emotional learning in the high school setting: An overview. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2024; 1(3):156-63. [Link]
- Poudel P. Prevalence and determinants of income among people with disabilities in Nepal: A cross-sectional study. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2025; 2(1):46-55. [Link]
- Rizzo A, Calandi L, Faranda M, Rosano MG, Carlotta V, Vinci E. Stigma against mental illness and mental health: The role of Social Media. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2025; 2(2):125-30. [DOI:10.1590/SciELOPreprints.8985]
- Bruno E, Turay T, Titi T. [The future of occupational health and safety system in Italy trough public actor’s coordination and school-based peer education programs (Italian)]. Italian Journal of Psychology and Occupational Health. 2023; 3(1):34-7. [Link]
- Maggio MG, Rizzo A, Stagnitti MC, Manuli A, Calabro’ RS. System usability, stress and mood among teachers using distance learning. Italian Journal of Psychology and Occupational Health. 2022; 2(2):117-24. [doi:10.23668/psycharchives.12650]
- Khabbache H, Ouazizi K, Ait Ali D, Cherqui A, Rizzo A, Tarchi L, et al. Cultural placebos from the wild in patients with mental disorders: The case of the Nour Association in Fez-Morocco. Iranian Rehabilitation Journal. 2024; 22(1):129-38. [DOI:10.32598/irj.22.1.2288.2]
- Chirico F, Capitanelli I, Taino G, Rizzo A, Cramarossa AA, Sacco A. [Risk factors and prevention measures for teachers: A literature review with a proposal for an occupational health surveillance protocol in schools (Italian)]. Italian Journal of Psychology and Occupational Health. 2024; 4(2):99-113. [Link]
- Mutongoza BH. Student-on-staff violence at South African universities: A qualitative study. Italian Journal of Psychology and Occupational Health. 2023; 3(2):50-6. [Link]
- Chirico F. The importance of moral clarity and humanity in advancing medical research. A-dvances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2025; 2(2):76-7. [Doi:10.5281/zenodo.12737550]
- Gharib M, Nematpoor M, Abbasabadi MM, Kazemi K, Morgan GA. Validation and reliability of the Persian version of the dimensions of Mastery Questionnaire (DMQ 18) infant version for assessing mastery motivation in early childhood. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2025; 2(2):117-24. [Link]
- Zhussipbek G. The human right to health: Empathy and universal human dignity versus apathetic capitalism. Advances in Medicine, Psychology, and Public Health. 2025; 2(1):60-71. [Link]